Advertisement

Distant neighbours: friends or foes? Eurasian beavers show context-dependent responses to simulated intruders

  • Anke Benten
  • Hannah B Cross
  • Helga V Tinnesand
  • Andreas Zedrosser
  • Frank RosellEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Neighbour-stranger discrimination is widespread in territorial animals, and depending on the relative threat posed by neighbours and strangers, residents commonly exhibit either the “dear enemy phenomenon” or the “nasty neighbour effect”. Different members of the same group may represent different threat levels, and the response of residents can be modified depending on, e.g. the sex and dominance status of the intruder. Neighbour-stranger discrimination is primarily investigated in neighbours with shared borders, and whether residents recognize their more distant neighbours remains unexplored. Here, using experimental scent marks, we investigated whether Eurasian beavers (Castor fiber) discriminate between distant neighbours (i.e. two territories away) and strangers (i.e. further away than the known dispersal distance). We tested the hypotheses that Eurasian beavers can discriminate between distant neighbours and strangers and that social status (i.e. sub-dominant or dominant) and sex of the intruder affect the responses of resident beavers. We predicted that resident beavers show the “dear enemy phenomenon” in response towards dominant distant neighbours due to their territory ownership and the “nasty neighbour effect” in response towards sub-dominants due to their likelihood to disperse. Sex of residents and social status of intruders were important in explaining territorial responses, with males exhibiting stronger responses to sub-dominant distant neighbours, especially males, than sub-dominant strangers. No such discrimination was found by females or between dominant distant neighbours and strangers. We suggest that the “nasty neighbour” response by male resident beavers towards sub-dominant distant neighbours relates to the relative threat levels due to repeated intrusions during dispersal attempts.

Significance statement

Territorial animals discriminate between neighbours and strangers to allocate aggressive behaviour to conspecific intruders. This neighbour-stranger discrimination has primarily been investigated between adjacent neighbours, but extraterritorial movements of residents into distant territories occur. Whether residents can discriminate between distant neighbours and strangers has only been studied in skylarks (Alauda arvensis), based on acoustic communication. Yet, it is unknown whether distant neighbours are perceived as neighbours or strangers based on olfactory recognition. Here we investigated whether Eurasian beavers (C. fiber) can discriminate between distant neighbours (i.e. two territories away) and strangers, based on olfactory scent samples including information on social status (i.e. dominant or sub-dominant) and sex of the intruder. Our results show that male residents showed a “nasty neighbour” response towards sub-dominant distant neighbours. These findings highlight the sensitivity of territorial mammals to the familiarity and social status of intruders.

Keywords

Beaver Dear enemy Nasty neighbour Stranger Territoriality 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We gratefully thank Christian A. Robstad, Patricia M. Graf and Yasmin Dawson for help in the field. We would also like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions to improve the quality of the paper.

Funding information

This study was funded by the University of South-Eastern Norway.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical statement

All capture and handling procedures performed in this study involving animals were approved by the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management (2012/1191 ART-VI-ORD) and the Norwegian Experimental Animal Board (ID 4387). The ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching were met (ASAB/ABS 2016). None of the animals captured for this study were injured, and all were successfully released at the site of capture after handling. No subsequent long-term effects of capture were observed.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

265_2019_2792_MOESM1_ESM.docx (19 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 18 kb)

References

  1. Akçay C, Wood WE, Searcy WA, Templeton CN, Campbell SE, Beecher MD (2009) Good neighbour, bad neighbour: song sparrows retaliate against aggressive rivals. Anim Behav 78:97–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnold TW (2010) Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike’s information criterion. J Wildl Manag 74:1175–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. ASAB/ABS (2016) Guidelines for the treatment of animal in behavioural research and teaching. Anim Behav 111:I–IX.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(15)00461-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barton K (2013) MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 1(43):6 http://www2.uaem.mx/r-mirror/web/packages/MuMIn. Accessed 22 Sept 2019
  5. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48.  https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, White JS (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 24:127–135PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Booksmythe I, Jennions MD, Backwell PRY (2010) Investigating the 'dear enemy' phenomenon in the territory defence of the fiddler crab, Uca mjoebergi. Anim Behav 7:419–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Briefer E, Aubin T, Rybak F (2009) Response to displaced neighbours in a territorial songbird with a large repertoire. Naturwissenschaften 96:1067–1077PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Briefer E, Rybak F, Aubin T (2010) Are unfamiliar neighbours considered to be dear-enemies? PLoS One 5:e12428PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Campbell R (2010) Demography and life history of the Eurasian beaver Castor fiber. PhD thesis, Merton College, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  11. Campbell RD, Rosell F, Nolet BA, Dijkstra VAA (2005) Territory and group sizes in Eurasian beavers (Castor fiber): echoes of settlement and reproduction? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58:597–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Campbell RD, Newman C, Macdonald DW, Rosell F (2013) Proximate weather patterns and spring green-up phenology effect Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber) body mass and reproductive success: the implications of climate change and topography. Glob Chang Biol 19:1311–1324PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carazo P, Font E, Desfilis E (2008) Beyond ‘nasty neighbours' and ‘dear enemies'? Individual recognition by scent marks in a lizard (Podarcis hispanica). Anim Behav 76:1953–1963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Christensen C, Kern JM, Bennitt E, Radford AN (2016) Rival group scent induces changes in dwarf mongoose immediate behavior and subsequent movement. Behav Ecol 27:1627–1634Google Scholar
  15. Crawford JC, Liu Z, Nelson TA, Nielsen CK, Bloomquist CK (2008) Microsatellite analysis of mating and kinship in beavers (Castor canadensis). J Mammal 89:575–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Crawley MJ (2007) The R book. Wiley, ChichesterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cross HB, Zedrosser A, Nevin O, Rosell F (2014) Sex discrimination via anal gland secretion in a territorial monogamous mammal. Ethology 120:1044–1052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. delBarco-Trillo J, McPhee ME, Johnston RE (2011) Syrian hamster males below an age threshold do not elicit aggression from unfamiliar adult males. Aggress Behav 37:91–97PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Eppley TM, Ganzhorn JU, Donati G (2016) Latrine behavior as a multimodal communicatory signal station in wild lemurs: the case of Hapalemur meridionalis. Anim Behav 111:57–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fawcett JK, Fawcett JM, Soulsbury CD (2013) Seasonal and sex differences in urine marking rates of wild red foxes Vulpes vulpes. J Ethol 31:41–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fisher RA (1954) Evolution and bird sociality. In: Huxley J, Hardy AC, Ford EB (eds) Evolution as a process. Allen & Unwin, London, pp 71–83Google Scholar
  22. Gilchrist JS, Otali E (2002) The effects of refuse-feeding on home-range use, group size, and intergroup encounters in the banded mongoose. Can J Zool 80:1795–1802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gosling L, Roberts S (2001) Scent marking by male mammal: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates. Adv Study Behav 30:169–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Graf PM, Wilson RP, Qasem L, Hackländer K, Rosell F (2015) The use of acceleration to code for animal behaviours: a case study in free-ranging Eurasian beavers Castor fiber. PLoS One 10:e0136751PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Graf PM, Mayer M, Zedrosser A, Häcklander K, Rosell F (2016) Territory size and age explain movement patterns in the Eurasian beaver. Mamm Biol 81:587–594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Grafen A (1987) The logic of divisively asymmetric contests: respect for ownership and the desperado effect. Anim Behav 35:462–467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Haughland DL, Larsen KW (2004) Exploration correlates with settlement: red squirrel dispersal in contrasting habitats. J Anim Ecol 73:1024–1034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Havens RP (2006) Beaver home ranges and movement patterns on the Embarras River watershed in east Central Illinois. MSc thesis, Eastern Illinois University, CharlestonGoogle Scholar
  29. Herr J, Müller-Schwarze D, Rosell F (2006) Resident beavers (Castor canadensis) do not discriminate between castoreum scent marks from simulated adult and subadult male intruders. Can J Zool 84:615–622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hohwieler K, Rosell F, Mayer M (2018) Scent-marking behavior by subordinate Eurasian beavers. Ethology 124:591–599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kroodsma DE, Byers BE, Goodale E, Johnson S, Liu W-C (2001) Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Anim Behav 61:1029–1033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mayeaux DJ, Johnston RE (2002) Discrimination of individual odours by hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) varies with the location of those odours. Anim Behav 64:269–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mayer M, Zedrosser A, Rosell F (2017a) When to leave: the timing of natal dispersal in a large, monogamous rodent, the Eurasian beaver. Anim Behav 123:375–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Mayer M, Zedrosser A, Rosell F (2017b) Extra-territorial movements differ between territory holders and subordinates in a large, monogamous rodent. Sci Rep 7:15261PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mech LD, Boitani L (2010) Wolves: behavior, ecology, and conservation. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  36. Monclús R, Saavedra I, de Miguel J (2014) Context-dependent responses to neighbours and strangers in wild European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behav Process 106:17–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Müller CA, Manser MB (2007) ‘Nasty neighbours’ rather than ‘dear enemies’ in a social carnivore. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:959–965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Nolet B, Rosell F (1994) Territoriality and time budgets in beavers during sequential settlement. Can J Zool 72:1227–1237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Noonan MJ, Markham A, Newman C, Trigoni N, Buesching CD, Ellwood SA, Macdonald DW (2014) Climate and the individual: inter-annual variation in the autumnal activity of the European badger (Meles meles). PLoS One 9:e83156PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Piechocki R (1977) Ökologische Todesursachenforschung am Elbebiber (Castor fiber albicus). Beitr Jagd Wildforsch 10:332–341Google Scholar
  41. R Core Team (2018) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna https://www.R-project.org. Accessed 22 Sept 2019
  42. Radford AN (2005) Group-specific vocal signatures and neighbour-stranger discrimination in the cooperatively breeding green wood hoopoe. Anim Behav 70:1227–1234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ralls K (1971) Mammalian scent marking. Science 171:443–449PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rayor LS (1985) Effects of habitat quality on growth, age of first reproduction, and dispersal in Gunnison prairie dogs (Cynomys gunnisoni). Can J Zool 63:2835–2840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rosell F, Bjørkøyli T (2002) A test of the dear enemy phenomenon in the Eurasian beaver. Anim Behav 63:1073–1078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rosell F, Steifetten Ø (2004) Subspecies discrimination in the Scandinavian beaver (Castor fiber): combining behavioral and chemical evidence. Can J Zool 82:902–909CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rosell F, Sun L (1999) Use of anal gland secretion to distinguish the two beaver species Castor canadensis and Castor fiber. Wildl Biol 5:119–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rosell F, Sundsdal L (2001) Odorant source used in Eurasian beaver territory marking. J Chem Ecol 27:2471–2491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rosell F, Thomsen LR (2006) Sexual dimorphism in territorial scent marking by adult Eurasian beavers (Castor fiber). J Chem Ecol 32:1301–1315PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rosell F, Bergan F, Parker H (1998) Scent-marking in the Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber) as a means of territory defense. J Chem Ecol 24:207–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rosell F, Johansen G, Parker H (2000) Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber) behavioral response to simulated territorial intruders. Can J Zool 78:931–935CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rosell F, Zedrosser A, Parker H (2010) Correlates of body measurements and age in Eurasian beaver from Norway. Eur J Wildl Res 56:43–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schradin C, Schneider C, Lindholm AK (2010) The nasty neighbour in the striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio) steals paternity and elicits aggression. Front Zool 7:19PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schulte B (1998) Scent marking and responses to male castor fluid by beavers. J Mammal 79:191–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sharpe F, Rosell F (2003) Time budgets and sex differences in the Eurasian beaver. Anim Behav 66:1059–1067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Soulsbury CD, Baker PJ, Iossa G, Harris S (2008) Fitness costs of dispersal in red foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1289–1298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Steyaert S, Zedrosser A, Rosell F (2015) Socio-ecological contexts other than sex affect resource selection in the obligate monogamous Eurasian beaver. Oecologia 179:1023–1032PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sun L, Müller-Schwarze D (1997) Sibling recognition in the beaver: a field test for phenotype matching. Anim Behav 54:493–502PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sun L, Müller-Schwarze D, Schulte B (2000) Dispersal pattern and effective population size of the beaver. Can J Zool 78:393–398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Svendsen GE (1989) Pair formation, duration of pair-bonds, and mate replacement in a population of beavers (Castor canadensis). Can J Zool 67:336–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Temeles EJ (1994) The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they “dear enemies”? Anim Behav 47:339–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tinnesand HV, Jojola S, Zedrosser A, Rosell F (2013) The smell of desperadoes? Beavers distinguish between dominant and subordinate intruders. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:895–904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tinnesand HV, Buesching CD, Noonan MJ, Newman C, Zedrosser A, Rosell F, Macdonald DW (2015) Will trespassers be prosecuted or assessed according to their merits? A consilient interpretation of territoriality in a group-living carnivore, the European badger (Meles meles). PLoS One 10:e0132432PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. White M, Swaisgood R, Zhang H (2003) Chemical communication in the giant panda (Ailuropoda melanoleuca): the role of age in the signaller and assessor. J Zool 259:171–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  66. Woodward RL Jr, Bartos K, Ferkin MH (2000) Meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) and prairie voles (M. ochrogaster) differ in their responses to over-marks from opposite-and same-sex conspecifics. Ethology 106:979–992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Ydenberg RC, Giraldeau L-A, Falls JB (1988) Neighbours, strangers, and the asymmetric war of attrition. Anim Behav 36:343–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Young AJ, Monfort SL (2009) Stress and the cause of extra-territorial movement in a social carnivore. Biol Lett 5:439–441PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Wildlife Research UnitAgricultural Centre Baden-WürttembergAulendorfGermany
  2. 2.Department of Natural Sciences and Environmental Health, Faculty of Technology, Natural Sciences and Maritime SciencesUniversity of South-Eastern NorwayNotoddenNorway
  3. 3.Department of Integrative Biology, Institute of Wildlife Biology and Game ManagementUniversity of Natural Resources and Applied Life Sciences, ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations