Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 67, Issue 9, pp 1499–1511 | Cite as

Variation in the temporal and spatial use of signals and its implications for multimodal communication

Original Paper

Abstract

The use of signals across multiple sensory modalities in communication is common in animals and plants. Determining the information that each signal component conveys has provided unique insights into why multimodal signals evolve. However, how these complex signals are assessed by receivers will also influence their evolution, a hypothesis that has received less attention. Here, we explore multimodal signal assessment in a closely related complex of island flycatchers that have diverged in visual and acoustic signals. Using field experiments that manipulated song and plumage colour, we tested if song, a possible long-range signal, is assessed before plumage colour in conspecific recognition. We find that divergent song and colour are assessed in sequence, and this pattern of sequential assessment is likely mediated by habitat structure and the extent of differences in signal characteristics. A broad survey of the literature suggests that many organisms from a wide range of taxa sequentially assess multimodal signals, with long-range signals attracting conspecifics for further assessment of close-range signals. Our results highlight the need to consider how signals are assessed when understanding multimodal signal evolution. Finally, given the results of our field experiments indicating sequential assessment of divergent song and colour in the recognition of conspecifics, we discuss the consequences of multimodal signal divergence for the origin of species, as changes in signals across different sensory modalities may influence the evolution of premating reproductive isolation.

Keywords

Multimodal signals Sequential assessment Sexual selection Monarcha castaneiventris Premating reproductive isolation 

References

  1. Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker MC (1991) Response to male indigo and lazuli buntings and their hybrids to song playback in allopatric and sympatric populations. Behaviour 119:225–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker MC, Baker AEM (1990) Reproductive behavior of female buntings: isolating mechanisms in a hybridizing pair of species. Evolution 44:332–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Balakrishnan CN, Sorenson MD (2006) Song discrimination suggests premating isolation among sympatric indigo bird species and host races. Behav Ecol 17:473–478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Balkenius A, Dacke M (2010) Flight behaviour of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta towards unimodal and multimodal targets. J Exp Biol 213:3741–3747PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barraclough TG, Harvey PH, Nee S (1995) Sexual selection and taxonomic diversity in passerine birds. Proc R Soc Lond B 259:211–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barry KL, Holwell GI, Herberstein ME (2008) Female praying mantids use sexual cannibalism as a foraging strategy to increase fecundity. Behav Ecol 19:710–715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Birkhead TR, Fletcher F, Pellatt EJ (1998) Sexual selection in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata: condition, sex traits and immune capacity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 44:179–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Borgia G (1995) Complex male display and female choice in the spotted bowerbird: specialized functions for different bower decorations. Anim Behav 49:1291–1301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boughman JW (2001) Divergent sexual selection enhances reproductive isolation in sticklebacks. Nature 411:944–948PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer, SunderlandGoogle Scholar
  12. Bro-Jørgensen J (2009) Dynamics of multiple signalling systems: animal communication in a world in flux. Trends Ecol Evol 292:292–300Google Scholar
  13. Brookfield HC (1969) Some notes on the climate of the British Solomon Islands. Philos T Roy Soc B 255:207–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bureau of Meteorology (1975) Tropical cyclones in the Northern Australian regions 1971–1972. Australian Government Publishing Service, Canberra, Australian Capital TerritoryGoogle Scholar
  15. Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol Rev 78:575–595PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Candolin U, Reynolds JD (2001) Sexual signaling in the European bitterling: females learn the truth by direct inspection of the resource. Behav Ecol 12:407–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Coleman SW, Patricelli GL, Borgia G (2004) Variable female preferences drive complex male displays. Nature 428:742–745PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Coyne JA, Orr HA (1989) Patterns of speciation in Drosophila. Evolution 43:362–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Diamond JM (2002) Dispersal, mimicry and geographic variation in Northern Melanesian birds. Pac Sci 56:1–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat 139:S125–S153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Filardi CE (2003) Speciation and the biogeographic history of tropical pacific flycatchers (genus Monarcha). Dissertation. University of Washington, SeattleGoogle Scholar
  22. Filardi CE, Smith CE (2005) Molecular phylogenetics of monarch flycatchers (genus Monarcha) with emphasis on Solomon Island endemics. Molec Phyl Evol 37:776–788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Filardi CE, Smith CE (2008) Social selection and geographic variation in two monarch flycatchers from the Solomon Islands. Condor 110:24–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gibson JS, Uetz GW (2008) Seismic communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: components of male seismic signals and mating success. Anim Behav 75:1253–1262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gibson R (1996) Female choice in sage grouse: the roles of attraction and active comparison. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39:55–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Goyret J, Kelber A, Pfaff M, Raguso RA (2009) Flexible responses to visual and olfactory stimuli by foraging Manduca sexta: larval nutrition affects adult behaviour. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2739–2745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Grafe TU, Preininger D, Sztatecsny M, Kasah R, Dehling JM, Proksch S, Hodl W (2012) Multimodal communication in a noisy environment: a case study of the Bornean rock frog Staurois parvus. PLoS One 7:e37965PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Grafe TU, Wagner TC (2007) Multimodal signaling in male and female foot-flagging frogs Staurois guttatus (Ranidae): an alerting function of calling. Ethology 113:772–781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Grant BR, Grant PR (2002a) Lack of premating isolation at the base of a phylogenetic tree. Am Nat 160:1–19PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Grant BR, Grant PR (2002b) Simulating secondary contact in allopatric speciation: an empirical test of premating isolation. Biol J Linn Soc 76:545–556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Guilford T, Dawkins MS (1991) Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim Behav 42:1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hankison SJ, Morris MR (2003) Avoiding a compromise between sexual selection and species recognition: female swordtail fish assess multiple species-specific cues. Behav Ecol 14:282–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hebets EA (2008) Seismic signal dominance in the multimodal courtship display of the wolf spider Schizocosa stridulans Stratton 1991. Behav Ecol 19:1250–1257PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hebets EA, Papaj DR (2005) Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:197–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hebets EA, Uetz GW (1999) Female responses to isolated signals from multimodal male courtship displays in the wolf spider genus Schizocosa (Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim Behav 57:865–872PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Irwin DE, Bensch S, Price TD (2001) Speciation in a ring. Nature 409:333–337PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kaczorowski RL, Leonard AS, Dornhaus A, Papaj DR (2012) Floral signal complexity as a possible adaptation to environmental variability: a test using nectar-foraging bumblebees, Bombus impatiens. Anim Behav 83:905–913CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kirkpatrick M, Ravigne V (2002) Speciation by natural and sexual selection: models and experiments. Am Nat 159:S22–S35PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kroodsma DE, Byers BE, Goodale E, Johnson S, Liu W (2001) Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Anim Behav 61:1029–1033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kulahci IG, Dornhaus A, Papaj DR (2008) Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-bees. Proc R Soc Lond B 275:797–802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lampe HM, Saetre G-P (1995) Female pied flycatchers prefer males with larger song repertoires. Proc R Soc Lond B 262:163–167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lande R (1981) Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 78:3721–3725PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lehtonen TK, Rintakoski S, Lindstrom K (2007) Mate preference for multiple cues: interplay between male and nest size in the sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutus. Behav Ecol 18:696–700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Leonard AS, Hedrick AV (2010) Long-distance signals influence assessment of close range mating displays in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Biol J Linn Soc 100:856–865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mayr E (1942) Systematics and the origin of species. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  46. McDonald DB (1989) Correlates of male mating success in a lekking bird. Anim Behav 37:1007–1022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McGregor PK, Catchpole C, Dabelsteen T, Falls JB, Fusani L et al (1992) Design of playback experiments: the Thornbridge Hall NATO ARW Consensus. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp 1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McLennan DA (2003) The importance of olfactory signals in the gasterosteid mating system: sticklebacks go multimodal. Biol J Linn Soc 80:555–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Partan S, Yelda S, Price V, Shimizu T (2005) Female pigeons, Columba livia, respond to multisensory audio/video playbacks of male courtship behaviour. Anim Behav 70:957–966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Partan SR, Marler P (2005) Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am Nat 166:231–245PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Patricelli GL, Uy JAC, Borgia G (2003) Multiple male traits interact: attractive bower decorations facilitate attractive behavioural displays in satin bowerbirds. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:2389–2395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Persons MH, Fleishman LJ, Frye MA, Stimphil ME (1999) Sensory response patterns and the evolution of visual signal design in anoline lizards. J Comp Physiol A 184:585–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Price T (2007) Speciation in birds. Roberts & Co, Greenwood VillageGoogle Scholar
  55. Price TD, Bouvier MM (2002) The evolution of F-1 postzygotic incompatibilities in birds. Evolution 56:2083–2089PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Rafferty NE, Boughman JW (2006) Olfactory mate recognition in a sympatric species pair of three-spined sticklebacks. Behav Ecol 17:965–970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Ramsey J, Bradshaw HD, Schemske DW (2003) Components of reproductive isolation between the monkey flowers Mimulus lewisii and M. cardinalis (Phrymacae). Evolution 57:1520–1534PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Ratcliffe LM, Grant PR (1985) Species recognition in Darwin’s finches (Geospiza, Gould).3. Male responses to playback of different song types, dialects and heterospecific songs. Anim Behav 33:290–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Ratti JT (1979) Reproductive separation and isolating mechanisms between sympatric dark- and light-phase Western Grebes. Auk 96:573–586Google Scholar
  60. Read JL (2011) The last wild island: saving tetepare. Page Digital, Kensington ParkGoogle Scholar
  61. Robson TE, Goldizen AW, Green DJ (2005) The multiple signals assessed by female satin bowerbirds: could they be used to narrow down females’ choices of mates? Biol Lett 1:264–267PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rosenthal GG, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2004) The vocal sac as a visual cue in anuran communication: an experimental analysis using video playback. Anim Behav 68:55–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Rybak F, Sureau G, Aubin T (2002) Functional coupling of acoustic and chemical signals in the courtship behaviour of the male Drosophila melanogaster. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:695–701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rypstra AL, Schlosser AM, Sutton PL, Persons MH (2009) Multmodal signalling: the relative importance of chemical and visual cues from females to the behaviour of male wolf spiders (Lycosidae). Anim Behav 77:937–947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Seddon N, Tobias JA (2007) Song divergence at the edge of Amazonia: an empirical test of the peripatric speciation model. Biol J Linn Soc 90:173–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Stoffer B, Walker SE (2012) The use of multimodal communication in mate choice decisions by female house crickets, Acheta domesticus. Anim Behav 83:1131–1138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Tack EJ, Putland DA, Robson TE, Goldizen AW (2005) Geographic variation in vocalisations of Satin Bowerbirds, Ptilonorynchus violaceus, in south-eastern Queensland. Emu 105:27–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Taylor RC, Buchanan BW, Doherty JL (2007) Sexual selection in the squirrel treefrog Hyla squirella: the role of multimodal cue assessment in female choice. Anim Behav 74:1753–1763CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Taylor RC, Klein BA, Stein J, Ryan MJ (2008) Faux frogs: multimodal signalling and the value of robotics in animal behaviour. Anim Behav 76:1089–1097CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Uetz GW, Roberts JA, Taylor PW (2009) Multimodal communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: female response to multimodal versus unimodal signals. Anim Behav 78:299–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Uy JAC, Moyle RG, Filardi CE (2009a) Plumage color and song differences mediate species recognition between incipient flycatcher species of the Solomon Islands. Evolution 63:153–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Uy JAC, Moyle RG, Filardi CE, Cheviron ZA (2009b) Difference in plumage color used in species recognition between incipient species is linked to a single amino acid substitution in the melanocortin-1 receptor. Am Nat 174:244–254PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual selection, social competition and speciation. Q Rev Biol 58:155–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Whitmore TC (1969) The vegetation of the Solomon Islands. Philos T Roy Soc B 255:259–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Wilkins MR, Seddon N, Safran RJ (2012) Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends Ecol Evol. doi 10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.002
  76. Winn HE, Winn LK (1978) The song of the humpback whale Megaptera novaeangliae in the West Indies. Mar Biol 47:97–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Zuk M, Rebar D, Scott SP (2008) Courtship song is more variable than calling song in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Anim Behav 76:1065–1071CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA
  2. 2.Department of Ecology and Evolutionary BiologyUniversity of ColoradoBoulderUSA

Personalised recommendations