Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 67, Issue 3, pp 439–447 | Cite as

Bright turquoise as an intraspecific signal in the chameleon grasshopper (Kosciuscola tristis)

  • Kate D. L. Umbers
  • Nikolai J. Tatarnic
  • Gregory I. Holwell
  • Marie E. Herberstein
Original Paper

Abstract

Bright colours often communicate important information between conspecifics. In sexually dichromatic species where males exhibit bright colours, two hypotheses are often invoked to explain the function of the colour. First, if a male’s bright colour contains information about his quality, females may prefer brighter males. Equally, male colour may reliably provide other males with information about fighting ability or resource holding potential. In such circumstances, brighter males may win altercations and/or males may use rival colour to assess their likelihood of winning an interaction. In the chameleon grasshopper (Kosciuscola tristis), males but not females turn bright turquoise when their body temperature exceeds 25 °C. In this study, we tested whether the turquoise phase of colour change has a signaling role in inter- and intrasexual contexts. We predicted that females would prefer bright turquoise males over dull males, but found no evidence from several choice experiments to support this hypothesis. We also predicted that brighter males would win more fights than duller males. Whilst we did not find that brighter males won more fights in staged experiments, we found that the brightness of males who chose to enter fights was significantly correlated with their opponents’ brightness. Our results suggest that the brightness of males’ turquoise phase may provide competitors with important information about their rival’s fighting ability.

Keywords

Colour change Brightness Alpine Male competition Female choice Sexual signal 

References

  1. Adamo SA, Ehgoetz K, Sangster C, Whitehorne I (2006) Signaling to the enemy? Body pattern expression and its response to external cues during hunting in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda). Biol Bull 210:192–200PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnott G, Elwood RW (2008) Assessment of fighting ability in animal contests. Anim Behav 77:991–1004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berthold G (1980) Microtubules in the epidermal cells of Carausius morosus, their pattern and relation to pigment migration. J Insect Physiol 26:421–425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boal JG, Shashar N, Grable MM, Vaughan KH, Loew ER, Hanlon RT (2004) Behavioral evidence for intraspecific signaling with achromatic and polarized light by cuttlefish (Mollusca: Cephalopoda). Behaviour 141:837–861CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bonduriansky R (2007) Sexual selection and allometry: a critical reappraisal of the evidence and ideas. Evolution 61:838–849PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bosi SG, Hayes J, Large MCJ, Poladian L (2008) Color, iridescence and thermoregulation in Lepidoptera. Appl Optics 47:5235–5241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Briscoe AD, Chittka L (2001) The evolution of color vision in insects. Ann Rev Ent 46:471–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buchner A, Erdfelder E, Faul F (1997) G*Power. University of Duesseldorf, DuesseldorfGoogle Scholar
  9. Casey TM (1981) Behavioral mechanisms of thermoregulation. Wiley, TorontoGoogle Scholar
  10. Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD (1979) The roaring of red deer Cervus elaphus and the evolution of honest advertisment. Behaviour 69:145–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  12. Conrad KF, Pritchard G (1989) Female dimorphism and physiological colour change in the damselfly Argia vivida Hagen (Odonata: coenagrionidae). Can J Zool 67:298–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cuadrado M (1998a) The use of yellow spot colors as a sexual receptivity signal in females of Chamaeleo chamaeleon. Herpetologica 54:395–402Google Scholar
  14. Cuadrado M (1998b) Models painted with female-like colors elicited courtship by male common chameleons: evidence for a courtship releaser. J Ethol 16:73–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cuadrado M (2000) Body colors indicate the reproductive status of female common chameleons: experimental evidence for the intersex communication function. Ethology 106:79–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dijkstra PD, Seehausen O, Groothuis TGG (2005) Direct male–male competition can facilitate invasion of new colour types in Lake Victoria cichlids. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58:136–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ekblom R, Sæther SA, Grahn M, Fiske P, Kålås JA, Höglund J (2004) Major histocompatibility complex variation and mate choice in a lekking bird, the great snipe (Gallinago media). Mol Ecol 13:3821–3828PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Elias DO, Kasumovic MM, Punzalan D, Andrade MCB, Mason AC (2008) Assessment during aggressive contests between male jumping spiders. Anim Behav 76:901–910PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat 139:S125–S153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Filshie BK, Day MF, Mercer EH (1975) Colour and colour change in the grasshopper, Kosciuscola tristis. J Insect Physiol 21:1763–1770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Forsman A (1997) Thermal capacity of different colour morphs in the pygmy grasshopper Tetrix subulata. Ann Zool Fenn 34:145–149Google Scholar
  22. Gillis JE (1982) Substrate color matching cues in the cryptic grasshopper Circotettix rabula rabula. Anim Behav 30:113–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hagelin JC, Ligon JD (2001) Female quail prefer testosterone-mediated traits, rather than the ornate plumage of males. Anim Behav 61:465–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hammerstein P, Parker GA (1982) The asymmetric was of attrition. J Theor Biol 96:647–682CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hauber ME, Sherman PW (2003) Designing and interpreting experimental tests of self-referent phenotype matching. Anim Cog 6:69–71Google Scholar
  26. Heinrich B (1993) The hot-blooded insects: strategies and mechanisms of thermoregulation. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  27. Henwood K (1975) A field-tested thermoregulation model for two diurnal Namib Desert tenebrionid beetles. Ecology 56:1329–1342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Herring PJ (1965) Blue pigment of a surface living oceanic copepod. Nature 205:103–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hettyey A, Herczeg G, Laurila A, Crochet PA, Merila J (2009) Body temperature, size, nuptial colouration and mating success in male moor frogs (Rana arvalis). Amphib Reptil 30:37–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hurd P (1997) Is signalling of fighting ability costlier for weaker individuals? J Theor Biol 184:83–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jennings DJ, Gammell MP, Carlin CM, Hayden TJ (2004) Effect of body weight, antler length, resource value and experience on fight duration and intensity in fallow deer. Anim Behav 68:213–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Jennions MD, Backwell PRY (1996) Residency and size affect fight duration and outcome in the fiddler crab Uca annulipes. Biol J Linn Soc 57:293–306Google Scholar
  33. Kasumovic MM, Elias DO, Punzalan D, Mason AC, Andrade MCB (2009) Experience affects the outcome of agonistic contests without affecting the selective advantage of size. Anim Behav 77:1533–1538PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kelso EC, Verrell PA (2002) Do male veiled chameleons, Chamaeleo calyptratus, adjust their courtship displays in response to female reproductive status? Ethology 108:495–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Key KHL, Day MF (1954a) A temperature-controlled physiological colour response in the grasshopper, Kosciuscola tristis Sjöst. (Orthoptera: Acrididae). Aust J Zool 2:309–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Key KHL, Day MF (1954b) The physiological mechanism of colour change in the grasshopper, Kosciuscola tristis Sjöst. (Orthoptera: acrididae). Aust J Zool 2:340–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Knowlton N, Keller B (1982) Symmetric fights as a measure of escalation potential in a symbiotic, territorial snapping shrimp. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10:289–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Langridge KV (2006) Symmetrical crypsis and asymmetrical signalling in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 273:959–967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ligon JD, Zwartjes PW (1995) Ornate plumage of male red junglefowl does not influence mate choice by females. Anim Behav 49:117–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. May ML (1979) Insect thermoregulation. Annu Rev Entomol 24:313–349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Maynard Smith J, Parker GA (1976) The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim Behav 24:159–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Møller AP, Alatalo RV (1999) Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:85–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Norman MD, Finn J, Tregenza T (1999) Female impersonation as an alternative reproductive strategy in giant cuttlefish. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 266:1347–1349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Osorio D, Vorobyev M (2005) Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: adaptations for luminance and colour vision. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol 272:1745–1752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Otte D (1970) A comparative study of communicative behavior in grasshoppers. Miscellaneous Publications of the Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan 141:1–168Google Scholar
  46. O’Farrell AF (1964) On physiological colour change in some Australian Odonata. J Ent Soc Aust (NSW) 1:5–12Google Scholar
  47. Palmer ME, Calvé MR, Adamo SA (2006) Response of female cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda) to mirrors and conspecifics: evidence for signaling in female cuttlefish. Anim Cog 9:151–155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Prum RO, Morrison RL, Eyck GR (1994) Ten Structural color production by constructive reflection from ordered collagen arrays in a bird (Philapitta castanea: eurylaimidae). J Morphol 222:61–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ries C, Spaethe J, Sztatecsny M, Strondl C, Hodl W (2008) Turning blue and ultraviolet: sex-specific colour change during the mating season in the Balkan moor frog. J Zool 276:229–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Simmons LW, Tomkins JL (1996) Sexual selection and the allometry of earwig forceps. Evol Ecol 10:97–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Stuart-Fox D, Moussalli A (2008a) Camouflage, communication and thermoregulation: lessons from colour changing organisms. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:463–470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stuart-Fox D, Moussalli A (2008b) Selection for social signalling drives the evolution of chameleon colour change. PLoS Biol 6:22–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Stuart-Fox D, Moussalli A, Whiting MJ (2008) Predator-specific camouflage in chameleons. Biol Lett 4:326–329PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sword GA, Simpson SJ (2000) Is there an intraspecific role for density-dependent colour change in the desert locust? Anim Behav 59:861–870PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Taylor PW, Elwood RW (2003) The mismeasure of animal contests. Anim Behav 65:1195–1202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Taylor PW, Hasson O, Clark DL (2001) Initiation and resolution of jumping spider contests: roles for size, proximity, and early detection of rivals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:403–413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Thomas L, Juanes F (1996) The importance of statistical power analysis: an example from animal behaviour. Anim Behav 52:856–859CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tichy H, Loftus R (1987) Response characteristics of a cold receptor in the stick insect Carausius morosus. J Comp Phys A 160:33–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Turner H, Firth D (2010) Bradley–Terry models in R: the BradleyTerry2 packageGoogle Scholar
  60. Umbers KDL (2011) Turn the temperature to turquoise: cues for colour change in the male chameleon grasshopper (Kosciuscola tristis) (Orthoptera: Acrididae). J Insect Physiol 57:1198–1204PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Umbers KDL, Herberstein ME, Madin JS (2012a) Colour in insect thermoregulation: empirical and theoretical tests in a colour-changing grasshopper. J Insect Physiol. doi:10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.10.016
  62. Umbers KDL, Tatarnic NJ, Herberstein ME (2012b) Ferocious fighting between male grasshoppers. PLoS One 7:e49600PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Velando A, Beamonte-Barrientos R, Torres RH (2006) Pigment-based skin colour in the blue-footed booby: an honest signal of current condition used by females to adjust reproductive investment. Oecologica 149:535–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Veron JEN (1973) The physiological control of the chromatophores of Austrolestes annulosus (Odonata). J Insect Physiol 19:1689–1703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Veron JEN (1974) The role of physiological colour change in the thermoregulation of Austrolestes annulosus (Selys) (Odonata). Aust J Zool 22:457–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Veron JEN, O’Farrell AF, Dixon B (1974) The fine structure of Odonata chromatophores. Tissue Cell 6:613–626PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Vigneron JP, Pasteels JM, Windsor DM, Vértesy Z, Rassart M, Seldrum T, Dumont J, Deparis O, Lousse V, Biró LP, Ertz D, Welch V (2007) Switchable reflector in the Panamanian tortoise beetle Charidotella egregia (Chrysomelidae: Cassidinae). Phys Rev E 76:031907CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wells MS (1988) Effects of body size and resource value on fighting behaviour in a jumping spider. Anim Behav 36:321–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kate D. L. Umbers
    • 1
    • 4
  • Nikolai J. Tatarnic
    • 2
    • 4
  • Gregory I. Holwell
    • 3
  • Marie E. Herberstein
    • 4
  1. 1.Research School of BiologyAustralian National UniversityCanberraAustralia
  2. 2.Evolution & Ecology Research CentreUniversity of New South WalesSydneyAustralia
  3. 3.School of Biological SciencesThe University of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand
  4. 4.Department of Biological SciencesMacquarie UniversityNorth RydeAustralia

Personalised recommendations