Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 66, Issue 11, pp 1485–1492 | Cite as

Sex-specific plasticity in brain morphology depends on social environment of the guppy, Poecilia reticulata

  • Alexander Kotrschal
  • Björn Rogell
  • Alexei A. Maklakov
  • Niclas Kolm
Original Paper

Abstract

The vertebrate brain is a remarkably plastic organ, which responds quickly to environmental changes. However, to date, studies investigating plasticity in brain morphology have focused mostly on the physical properties of the surrounding environment, and little is known about brain plasticity in response to the social environment. Moreover, sex differences in brain plasticity remain virtually unexplored. Here, we tested how the social environment influenced brain morphology in adult males and females using experimental manipulation of the sex composition of social pairs (same sex vs. mixed sex) in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). We detected substantial sex-specific plasticity in both the overall brain size (controlling for body size) and separate brain structures. The brain size was larger in males that interacted with females, and female optic tectum was larger in female-only groups. Overall, females had larger olfactory bulbs and cerebellum in comparison to males. While net sexual dimorphism in the brain structure can be explained in light of the known differences in boldness and foraging behaviour between the sexes, our results also support that cognitive demands associated with courtship behaviour can lead to plastic changes in the brain size. Our findings demonstrate that not only social environment can generate rapid, plastic responses in the vertebrate brain but also that such responses can depend strongly on sex.

Keywords

Neural development Plasticity Social interactions Sexual selection Sexual dimorphism Guppy Poecilia reticulata 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by grants from the Swedish Research Council (for NK and AAM), Carl Trygger Foundation (for NK) and ERC (for AAM).

Ethical standards

All performed experiments comply with the Swedish law and were approved by the Uppsala ethics committee.

References

  1. Aiello LC, Wheeler P (1995) The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol 36:199–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  3. Arnqvist G, Rowe L (2005) Sexual conflict. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  4. Bell AM, Foster SA (1994) The evolutionary biology of the threespine stickleback. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc B 57:289–300Google Scholar
  6. Braithwaite VA (2006) Cognitive ability in fish. In: Sloman KA, Wilson RW, Balshine S (eds) Behaviour and physiology of fish. Fish physiology, vol 24. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 1–38Google Scholar
  7. Burns JG, Saravanan A, Rodd FH (2009) Rearing environment affects the brain size of guppies: lab-reared guppies have smaller brains than wild-caught guppies. Ethology 115:122–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Butler AB, Hodos W (2005) Comparative vertebrate neuroanatomy. Wiley, HobokenCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Croft DP, Morrell LJ, Wade AS, Piyapong C, Ioannou CC, Dyer JRG, Chapman BB, Yan W, Krause J (2006) Predation risk as a driving force for sexual segregation: a cross-population comparison. Am Nat 167:867–878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. De Groof G, Verhoye M, Van Meir V, Balthazart J, Van der Linden A (2008) Seasonal rewiring of the songbird brain: an in vivo MRI study. Europ J Neurosci 28:2475–2485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. De Groof G, Verhoye M, Poirier C, Leemans A, Eens M, Darras VM, Van der Linden A (2009) Structural changes between seasons in the songbird auditory forebrain. J Neurosci 29:13557–13565PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Evans JP, Magurran AE (1999) Male mating behaviour and sperm production characteristics under varying sperm competition risk in guppies. Anim Behav 58:1001–1006PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Forstmeier W (2011) Women have relatively larger brains than men: a comment on the misuse of general linear models in the study of sexual dimorphism. Anat Rec 294:1856–1863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gasparini C, Devigili A, Pilastro A (2012) Cross-generational effects of sexual harassment on female fitness in the guppy. Evolution 66:532–543PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gittleman JL (1994) Female brain size and parental care in carnivores. P Natl Acad Sci USA 91:5495–5497CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gonda A, Herczeg G, Merilä J (2009) Habitat-dependent and -independent plastic responses to social environment in the nine-spined stickleback (Pungitius pungitius) brain. Proc Roy Soc B 276:2085–2092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gonda A, Trokovic N, Herczeg G, Laurila A, Merilä J (2010) Predation- and competition-mediated brain plasticity in Rana temporaria tadpoles. J Evol Biol 23:2300–2308PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gonzalez-Voyer A, Kolm N (2010) Sex, ecology and the brain: evolutionary correlates of brain structure volumes in Tanganyikan cichlids. PLoS One 5:e14355PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gonzalez-Voyer A, Winberg S, Kolm N (2009) Social fishes and single mothers: brain evolution in African cichlids. Proc Roy Soc B 276:161–167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Griffiths SW (1996) Sex differences in the trade-off between feeding and mating in the guppy. J Fish Biol 48:891–898CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Healy SD, Rowe C (2007) A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proc Roy Soc B 274:453–464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hofman MA, Swaab DF (2002) A brain for all season: cellular and molecular mechanism of photoperiodic plasticity. In: Hofman MA, Boer GJ, Holtmaat GD, van Someren EJW, Verhaagen J, Swaab DF (eds) Plasticity of the adult brain. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 255–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Houde A (1997) Sex, color, and mate choice in guppies. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  24. Huber R, vanStaaden MJ, Kaufman LS, Liem KF (1997) Microhabitat use, trophic patterns, and the evolution of brain structure in African cichlids. Brain Behav Evolut 50:167–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hutcheon JM, Kirsch JW, Garland T (2002) A comparative analysis of brain size in relation to foraging ecology and phylogeny in the chiroptera. Brain Behav Evolut 60:165–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jacobs LF (1996) Sexual selection and the brain. Trends Ecol Evol 11:82–86PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jacobs LF, Gaulin SJC, Sherry DF, Hoffman GE (1990) Evolution of spatial cognition - sex-specific patterns of spatial-behavior predict hippocampal size. P Natl Acad Sci USA 87:6349–6352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kihslinger RL, Nevitt GA (2006) Early rearing environment impacts cerebellar growth in juvenile salmon. J Exp Biol 209:504–509PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kotrschal A, Räsänen K, Kristjansson BK, Senn M, Kolm N (2012a) Extreme sexual brain size dimorphism in sticklebacks: a consequence of the cognitive challenges of sex and parenting? PLoS One 7:e30055PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kotrschal A, Sundström LF, Brelin D, Devlin RH, Kolm N (2012b) Inside the heads of David and Goliath: environmental effects on brain morphology among wild and growth-enhanced coho salmon, Oncorhynchus kisutch. J Fish Biol 81:987–1002PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Laland KN, Reader SM (1999a) Foraging innovation in the guppy. Anim Behav 57:331–340PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Laland KN, Reader SM (1999b) Foraging innovation is inversely related to competitive ability in male but not in female guppies. Behav Ecol 10:270–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lessells CM, Boag PT (1987) Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk 104:116–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Liley NR (1966) Ethological isolating mechanisms in four sympatric species of poeciliid fishes. Behaviour, Suppl 13:1–197Google Scholar
  35. Magurran AE (2005) Evolutionary ecology: the Trinidadian guppy. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Magurran AE, Seghers BH (1994) A cost of sexual harassment in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 258:89–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C (1998) The cenrtral nervous system of vertebrates. Springer, HeidelbergGoogle Scholar
  38. Nottebohm F (1981) A brain for all seasons: cyclical anatomical changes in song control. Science 214:1368–1370PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Nottebohm F, Arnold AP (1976) Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of songbird brain. Science 194:211–213PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pilastro A, Mandelli M, Gasparini C, Dadda M, Bisazza A (2007) Copulation duration, insemination efficiency and male attractiveness in guppies. Anim Behav 74:321–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pitnick S, Miller GT (2000) Correlated response in reproductive and life history traits to selection on testis length in Drosophila hydei. Heredity 84:416–426PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pitnick S, Jones KE, Wilkinson GS (2006) Mating system and brain size in bats. Proc Roy Soc B 273:719–724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Piyapong C, Krause J, Chapman BB, Ramnarine IW, Louca V, Croft DP (2010) Sex matters: a social context to boldness in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol 21:3–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Pollen AA, Dobberfuhl AP, Scace J, Igulu MM, Renn SCP, Shumway CA, Hofmann HA (2007) Environmental complexity and social organization sculpt the brain in Lake Tanganyikan cichlid fish. Brain Behav and Evol 70:21–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pravosudov VV, Clayton NS (2002) A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behav Neurosci 116:515–522PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. P Natl Acad Sci USA 99:4436–4441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Reznick D, Yang AP (1993) The influence of fluctuating resources on life-history: patterns of allocation and plasticity in female guppies. Ecology 74:2011–2019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rodriguez F, Duran E, Gomez A, Ocana FM, Alvarez E, Jimenez-Moya F, Broglio C, Salas C (2005) Cognitive and emotional functions of the teleost fish cerebellum. Brain Res Bull 66:365–370PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL (1996) Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res 78:57–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Striedter GF (2005) Principles of brain evolution. Sinauer Associates, SunderlandGoogle Scholar
  51. Ullmann JFP, Cowin G, Collin SP (2010) Magnetic resonance microscopy of the barramundi (Lates calcarifer) brain. J Morphol 271:1446–1456PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. van Staaden MJ, Huber R, Kaufmann LS, Liem KF (1995) Brain evolution in cichlids of the African Great Lakes: brain and body size, general patterns and evolutionary trends. Zoology 98:165–178Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alexander Kotrschal
    • 1
  • Björn Rogell
    • 1
  • Alexei A. Maklakov
    • 1
  • Niclas Kolm
    • 1
  1. 1.Animal Ecology, Department of Ecology and GeneticsUppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden

Personalised recommendations