Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 65, Issue 4, pp 717–726 | Cite as

Discrimination of vocal performance by male swamp sparrows

  • Adrienne L. DuBoisEmail author
  • Stephen Nowicki
  • William A. Searcy
Original Paper

Abstract

In aggressive communication, the interests of signalers and receivers are directly opposed, presenting a challenge to the maintenance of reliable signaling. Index signals, whose production is constrained by physical ability, offer one solution to the reliable signaling problem. Vocal performance, the ability to produce physically challenging songs, is likely such a signal in swamp sparrows. Maximum vocal performance varies between males and is correlated with aspects of quality. However, vocal performance can be modulated in aggressive contexts by increasing the frequency bandwidth and trill rate of songs. This study examines receiver response to (1) differences in performance of the same song types by different signalers and (2) individual modulation of performance between contexts. Results demonstrate that male receivers show differential response to between-male differences in song type performance, but do not show differential response to the smaller scale modulations of performance produced by individuals singing the same song type at different times. This pattern suggests that vocal performance cannot be effectively cheated and may therefore serve as a good example of an index signal.

Keywords

Birdsong Vocal performance Aggression Reliable signaling Communication 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Barbara Ballentine for providing stimuli for the inter-male discrimination experiment, Susan Peters for her gracious and invaluable help in creating artificial stimuli for the intra-male discrimination experiment, and Kennon Todd for assistance in the field during pilot work for this study. We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions in revising the manuscript. We would also like to thank the Pymatuning Laboratory of Ecology for logistical support and the Pennsylvania Game Commission for access to study sites. This work was funded by the University of Miami College of Arts and Sciences and the University of Pittsburgh’s Arthur and Barbara Pape Endowment.

Ethical standards

The experimental methods for this study complied with current rules and regulations within the USA.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ballentine B (2009) The ability to perform physically challenging songs predicts age and size in male swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana. Anim Behav 77:973–978CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ballentine B, Hyman J, Nowicki S (2004) Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav Ecol 15:163–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ballentine B, Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2008) Reliable aggressive signaling in swamp sparrows. Anim Behav 75:693–703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bee MA, Perrill SA (1996) Responses to conspecific advertisement calls in the green frog (Rana clamitans) and their role in male-male communication. Behav 133:283–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bee MA, Perrill SA, Owen PC (1999) Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana clamitans). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 45:177–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beebee MD (2004) Variation in vocal performance in the songs of a wood-warbler: evidence for the function of distinct singing modes. Ethol 110:531–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cardoso GC, Atwell JW, Ketterson ED, Price TD (2007) Inferring performance in the songs of dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Behav Ecol 18:1051–1057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cardoso GC, Atwell JW, Ketterson ED, Price TD (2009) Song types, song performance, and the use of repertoires in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Behav Ecol 20:901–907CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K (2007) Female perception of size-related formant shifts in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Behav 74:707–714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cramer ERA, Price J (2007) Red-winged blackbirds Ageliaus phoeniceus respond differently to song types with different performance levels. J Avian Biol 38:122–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Davies NB, Halliday TR (1978) Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274:683–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1978) Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 282–309Google Scholar
  13. Draganoiu TI, Nagle L, Kreutzer M (2002) Directional female preference for an exaggerated male trait in canary (Serinus canaria) song. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:2525–2531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. DuBois AL, Nowicki S, Searcy WA (2009) Swamp sparrows modulate vocal performance in an aggressive context. Biol Lett 5:163–165PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fitch WT (1997) Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. J Acoust Soc Am 102:1213–1222PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fitch WT, Reby D (2001) The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1669–1675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Forstmeier W, Kempenauers B, Meyer A, Leisler B (2002) A novel song parameter correlates with extra-pair paternity and reflects male longevity. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:1479–1485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Grafen A (1990) Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol 144:517–546PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hoese WJ, Podos J, Boetticher NC, Nowicki S (2000) Vocal tract function in birdsong production: experimental manipulation of beak movements. J Exp Biol 203:1845–1855PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Holveck MJ, Riebel K (2007) Preferred songs predict preferred males: consistency and repeatability of zebra finch females across three test contexts. Behav 74:297–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hurd PL, Enquist M (2005) A strategic taxonomy of biological communication. Behav 70:1155–1170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Illes AE, Hall MH, Vehrencamp S (2006) Vocal performance influences male receiver response in the banded wren. Proc R Soc Lond B 273:1907–1912CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Koivula K, Lahti K, Orell M, Rytkönen S (1993) Prior residency as a key determinant of social dominance in the willow tit (Parus montanus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33:283–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Leadbeater E, Goller F, Riebel K (2005) Unusual phonation, covarying song characteristics and song preferences in female zebra finches. Behav 70:909–919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Martin WF (1972) Evolution of vocalization in the genus Bufo. In: Blair WF (ed) Evolution in the Genus Bufo. University of Texas, Austin, pp 279–309Google Scholar
  26. Maynard Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal signals. New York, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  27. Nowicki S (1987) Vocal tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: evidence from birdsongs in a helium atmosphere. Nature 325:53–55PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nowicki S, Marler P (1988) How do birds sing? Music Percept 5:391–426Google Scholar
  29. Podos J (1997) A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51:537–551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Podos J (2001) Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches. Nature 409:185–188PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Podos J, Nowicki S (2004a) Beaks, adaptation, and vocal evolution in Darwin’s finches. BioSci 54:501–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Podos J, Nowicki S (2004b) Performance limits on birdsong. In: Marler P, Slabbekorn H (eds) Nature’s music. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 318–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Reby D, McComb K (2003) Anatomical constraints generate honesty: acoustic cues to age and weight in the roars of red deer stags. Anim Behav 65:519–530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Reby D, McComb K, Cargnelutti B, Darwin C, Fitch WT, Clutton-Brock TH (2005) Red deer stags use formants as assessment cues during intrasexual agonistic interactions. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:941–947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Richner H (1989) Phenotypic correlates of dominance in carrion crows and their effects on access to food. Anim Behav 38:606–612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ryan MJ (1985) The Tungara frog: a study in sexual selection and communication. University of Chicago, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  37. Sandell M, Smith HG (1991) Dominance, prior occupancy, and winter residency in the great tit (Parus major). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:147–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Searcy WA (1979) Morphological correlates of dominance in captive male red-winged blackbirds. Condor 81:417–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Searcy WA, Beecher MD (2009) Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds. Anim Behav 78:1281–1292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  41. Searcy WA, Podos J, Peters S, Nowicki S (1995) Discrimination of song types and variants in song sparrows. Anim Behav 49:1219–1226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Searcy WA, Nowicki S, Peters S (1999) Song types as fundamental units in vocal repertoires. Anim Behav 58:37–44PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Vehrencamp S (2000) Handicap, index, and conventional signal elements of bird song. In: Espmark Y, Amundsen T, Rosenqvist G (eds) Signalling and signal design in animal communication. Tapir, Trondheim, pp 301–315Google Scholar
  44. Wagner WE (1989a) Fighting, assessment, and frequency alteration in Blanchard’s cricket frog. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:429–436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wagner WE (1989b) Social correlates of variation in male calling behavior in Blanchard’s cricket frog, Acris crepitans blanchardi. Ethol 82:27–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wagner W (1992) Deceptive or honest signaling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. Anim Behav 44:449–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Westneat MW, Long JH, Hoese W, Nowicki S (1993) Kinematics of birdsong: functional correlation of cranial movements and acoustic features in sparrows. J Exp Biol 182:141–171Google Scholar
  48. Yamaguchi N, Kawano KK (2001) Effect of body size on the resource holding potential of male varied tits Parus varius. Jpn J Ornithol 50:65–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Adrienne L. DuBois
    • 1
    Email author
  • Stephen Nowicki
    • 2
  • William A. Searcy
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA
  2. 2.Department of BiologyDuke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations