Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 64, Issue 6, pp 1001–1009 | Cite as

Digit ratio (2D:4D) and dominance rank in female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)

  • Emma Nelson
  • Christy L. Hoffman
  • Melissa S. Gerald
  • Susanne Shultz
Original Paper

Abstract

Female rhesus macaques exhibit matrilineal dominance structures, and high dominance rank confers fitness benefits across a lifetime and across generations. Rank effects are “inherited” through social processes that are well understood; however, biological mechanisms that might impact these processes are not well known. Recently, it has been shown that prenatal androgens appear to be implicated in supporting dominance rank hierarchies in some mammals. In humans, interindividual differences in the second (index) to fourth (ring) digit ratio (2D:4D) have been linked indirectly to variation in prenatal androgens, with low 2D:4D in both sexes associated with higher inferred prenatal androgen effects. 2D:4D has also been related to dominant social behavior and has been shown to co-vary with social systems across nonhuman primate species. Here, we investigate how 2D:4D co-varies with socially inherited dominance rank in female rhesus macaques. Low 2D:4D was associated with higher-ranking females, while higher 2D:4D was associated with lower-ranking females. Similar relationships were also shown between ranked families within matrilines. This is the first study to show such a relationship between 2D:4D and dominance rank in a nonhuman primate and suggests that prenatal androgen effects could be involved in the maintenance of dominance rank in female cercopithecine primates.

Keywords

Primates Prenatal androgens Prenatal programming Competition Aggression Dominance rank 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the staff at the Caribbean Primate Research Center, Cayo Santiago for their support and help. We also thank Dario Maestripieri and Anja Widdig for kindly allowing access to their subjects to collect data and Doreen Hess, Constance Dubuc, and Julie Cascio for help with data collection. We would also like to thank Martin Voracek, Kobe Millet, and three anonymous reviewers who helped improve the manuscript. The protocol for this study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, Medical Sciences Department, University of Puerto Rico. This publication was made possible by Grant Number P40RR003640 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NCRR or NIH. Funding support was provided by the University of Liverpool, the British Academy Centenary Project, and the British Association of Biological Anthropology and Osteoarchaeology.

References

  1. Altmann J, Ward Lynch J, Nguyen N, Alberts SC, Gesquiere LR (2004) Life-history correlates of steroid concentrations in wild peripartum baboons. Am J Primatol 64:95–106CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2004) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Biol Psychol 68:215–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bastian ML, Sponberg AC, Suomi SJ, Higley JD (2003) Long-term effects of infant rearing condition on the acquisition of dominance rank in juvenile and adult rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Dev Psychobiol 42:44–51CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Beehner JC, Phillips-Conroy JE, Whitten PL (2005) Female testosterone, dominance rank, and aggression in an Ethiopian population of hybrid baboons. Am J Primatol 67:101–119CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Benderlioglu Z, Nelson RJ (2004) Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but not men. Horm Behav 46:558–564CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown WH, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM (2002) Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav 42:380–386CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Buck JJ, Williams RM, Hughes IA, Acerini CL (2003) In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio—comparisons between healthy controls and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hum Reprod 18:976–979CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Carbone JP, Figurska K, Buck S, Brent RL (2005) Effect of gestational sex steroid exposure on limb development and endochondral ossification in the pregnant C57BI/6 J mouse: I. Medroxyprogesterone acetate. Teratology 42:121–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Castracane VD, Stewart DR, Gimpel T, Overstreet JW, Lasley BL (1998) Maternal serum androgens in human pregnancy early increase within the cycle of conception. Hum Reprod 13:460–464CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cattrall FR, Vollenhoven BJ, Weston GC (2005) Anatomical evidence for in utero androgen exposure in women with polycystic ovary syndrome. Fertil Steril 84:1689–1692CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chapais B (1992) The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among female primates. In: Harcourt A, de Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 29–59Google Scholar
  12. Chapais B (2004) How kinship generates dominance structures: a comparative perspective. In: Thierry B, Singh W, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 186–203Google Scholar
  13. Chauvin C, Berman CM (2004) Intergenerational transmission of behavior. In: Thierry B, Singh W, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 209–230Google Scholar
  14. Chepko-Sade D, Sade DS (1979) A study of social group structure in Macaca mulatta (Cercopithecidae: Primates). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5:67–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Clark AP (2004) Self-perceived attractiveness and masculinization predict women’s sociosexuality. Evol Hum Behav 25:113–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Coates JM, Gurnell M, Rustichini A (2009) Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 13:623–628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Danforth JW (1973) Androgen-induce social dominance in infant female rhesus monkeys. J Child Psychol 14:37–145Google Scholar
  18. Datta SB (1988) The acquisition of dominance among free-ranging rhesus monkey siblings. Anim Behav 36:754–772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dloniak SM, French JA, Holekamp KE (2006) Rank-related maternal effects of androgens on behaviour in wild spotted hyaenas. Nature 440:1190–1193CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Drummond H, Canales C (1998) Dominance between booby nestlings involves winner and loser effects. Anim Behav 55:1669–1676CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. East ML, Höner OP, Watcher B, Wilhelm K, Burke T, Hofer H (2009) Maternal effects on offspring social status in spotted hyenas. Behav Ecol 20:478–483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Engh AL, Esch K, Smale L, Holekamp KE (2000) Mechanisms of maternal rank ‘inheritance’ in spotted hyena, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav 60:323–332CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Fink B, Thanzami V, Seydel H, Manning JT (2006) Digit ratio and hand-grip strength in German and Mizos men: cross-cultural evidence for an organizing effect of prenatal testosterone on strength. Am J Hum Biol 18:776–782CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Forstmeier W (2005) Quantitative genetics and behavioural correlates of digit ratio in the zebra finch. Proc R Soc B 272:2641–2649CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gil D, Leboucher G, Lacroix A, Cue R, Kreutzer M (2004) Female canaries produce eggs with greater amounts of testosterone when exposed to preferred male song. Horm Behav 45:64–70CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gillam L, McDonald R, Ebling FJP, Mayhew TM (2008) Human 2D (index) and 4D(ring) finger lengths and ratios: cross-sectional data on linear growth patterns, sexual dimorphism and lateral asymmetry from 5 to 60 years of age. J Anat 213:325–335CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Gobrogge KL, Breedlove SM, Klump KL (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on 2D:4D finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins. Arch Sex Behav 37:112–118CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Goy RW, McEwen BS (1980) Sexual differentiation of the brain. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, MassachusettsGoogle Scholar
  29. Grafen A, Hails R (2002) Modern statistics for life sciences. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  30. Groothuis TGG, Müller W, von Engelhardt N, Carere C, Eising C (2005) Maternal hormones as a tool to adjust offspring phenotype in avian species. Neurosci Biobehav R 29:329–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Harris EF, Aksharanugraha K, Behrents RG (1992) Metacarpophalangeal length changes in humans during adulthood: a longitudinal study. Am J Phys Anthopol 87:263–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Higley JD, King ST, Hasert MF, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M (1996) Stability and interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in female rhesus macaques. Neuropsychopharmacology 14:67–76CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1991) Dominance acquisition during mammalian social development: the “inheritance” of maternal rank. Am Zool 31:306–317Google Scholar
  34. Hönekopp J, Manning JT, Muller C (2006) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and physical fitness in males and females: evidence for effects of prenatal androgens on sexually selected traits. Horm Behav 49:45–549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S (2006) mFetal testosterone and sex differences in typical development and in autism. J Child Neurol 21:825–845CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004) 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev 77:23–28CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Maestripieri D (1994) Costs and benefits of maternal aggression in lactating female Rhesus macaques. Primates 35:443–453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Maestripieri D (2003) Similarities in affiliation and aggression between cross-fostered rhesus macaque females and their biological infants. Dev Psychobiol 43:321–327CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Maestripieri D (2007) Macachiavellian intelligence: how rhesus macaques and humans have conquered the world. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  40. Malas MA, Dogan S, Hilal Evicil E, Desdicioglu K (2006) Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio. Early Hum Dev 82:469–475CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Manning JT (2002) Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, health, and behavior. Rutgers University Press, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  42. Manning JT, Fink B (2008) Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol 20:451–461CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Manning JT, Scutt D, Lewis-Jones DI (1998) The ratio of the 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and the concentration of testosterone, luteinizing hormones and oestrogen. Hum Reprod 13:3000–3004CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Manning JT, Churchill AJG, Peters M (2007a) The effects of sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav 36:223–233CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Manning JT, Morris L, Caswell N (2007b) Endurance running and digit ratio (2D:4D): implications for fetal testosterone effects on running speed and vascular health. Am J Hum Biol 19:416–421CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. McIntyre MH, Herrmann E, Wobber V, Halbwax M, Mohamba C, de Sousa N, Atencia R, Cox D, Hare B (2009) Bonobos have a more human-like second-to-fourth finger length ratio (2D:4D) than chimpanzees: a hypothesized indication of lower prenatal androgens. J Hum Evol 56:361–365CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Medland SE, Loehlin JC (2008) Multivariate genetic analyses of the 2D:4D ratio: examining the effects of hand and measurement technique in data from 757 twin families. Twin Res Hum Genet 11:335–341CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Medland SE, Loehlin JC, Martin NG (2008) No effect of prenatal hormone transfer on digit ratio in a large sample of same- and opposite-sex dizygotic twins. Pers Individ Diff 44:1225–1234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mehta PH, Josephs RA (2006) Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again. Horm Behav 50:684–692CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Millet K, Dewitte S (2007) Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the impact of an aggressive music video on aggression. Pers Individ Dif 43:289–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Millet K, Dewitte S (2008) A subordinate status position increases the present value of financial resources in low 2D:4D men. Am J Hum Biol 20:110–115CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Millet K, Dewitte S (2009) The presence of aggression cues inverts the relation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and pro-social behaviour in the dictator game. Brit J Psychol 100:151–162CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Mousseau TA, Fox CW (1998) Maternal effects as adaptations. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  54. Neave N, Laing S, Fink B, Manning JT (2003) Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. Proc R Soc B 270:2167–2172CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Nelson E, Shultz S (2010) Finger length ratios (2D:4D) in anthropoids implicate reduced prenatal androgens in social bonding. Am J Phys Anthropol 141:395–405PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Nelson E, Voracek M (2010) Heritability of digit ratio (2D:4D) in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Primates 51:1–5CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Nora JJ, Nora AH, Perinchief AG, Ingram JW, Fountain AK, Peterson MJ (1976) Congenital abnormalities and first-trimester exposure to progesterone/oestrogen. Lancet 1:313–314CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Ökten A, Kalyoncu M, Yaris N (2002) The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Hum Dev 70:47–54CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Ostner J, Heistermann M, Kappeler PM (2003) Intersexual dominance, masculinized genitals and prenatal steroids: comparative data from lemurid primates. Naturwissenschaften 90:141–144PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Paul SN, Kato BS, Cherkas LF, Toby A, Spector TD (2006) Heritability of the second to fourth digit ratio (2d:4d): a twin study. Twin Res Hum Genet 9:215–219CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Roney J, Maestripieri D (2004) Relative digit length predict men’s behavior and attractiveness during social interactions with women. Hum Nature 15:271–282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Saino N, Leoni B, Romano M (2006) Human digit ratios depend on birth order and sex or older siblings and predict maternal fecundity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:34–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Sapolsky RM (2005) The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science 308:648–652CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Sayer AA, Barker D (2002) The early environment, developmental plasticity and aging. Rev Clin Gerontol 12:205–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Schino G (2001) Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 62:265–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Schwabl H (1993) Yolk is a source of maternal testosterone for developing birds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:11446–11450CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Soto AM, Sonnenschein C (1999) Estrogens, xenoestrogens, and the development of neoplasms. In: Naz RK (ed) Endocine disruptors. CRC Press, Florida, pp 175–216Google Scholar
  68. Stucki BR, Dow MM, Sade DS (1991) Variance in intrinsic rates of growth among free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Am J Phys Anthropol 128:181–191Google Scholar
  69. Thierry B, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM (2000) The influence on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca). Ethology 106:713–728CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. van Anders SM (2007) Grip strength and digit ratios are not correlated in women. Am J Hum Biol 19:437–439CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. van Anders SM, Vernon SM, Wilbur CJ (2006) Finger length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone transfer between opposite-sex twins. Horm Behav 49:315–319CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Voracek M, Dressler SG (2007) Digit ratio (2D:4D) in twins: heritability estimates and evidence for a masculinized trait expression in women from opposite-sex pairs. Psychol Rep 100:115–126CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Voracek M, Dressler SG (2009) Familial resemblance in digit ratio (2D:4D). Am J Phys Anthropol 140:376–380CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Voracek M, Manning JT, Dressler SG (2007) Repeatability and interobserver error of digit ratio (2D:4D) measurements made by experts. Am J Hum Biol 19:142–146CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Wallen K (1996) Nature needs nurture: the interaction of hormonal and social influences on the development of behavioral sex differences in rhesus monkeys. Horm Behav 30:364–378CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Wallen K (2005) Hormonal influences on sexually differentiated behavior in nonhuman primates. Front Neuroendocrin 26:7–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wallen K (2009) Does finger fat produce sex differences in second to fourth digit ratios? Endocrinology 150:4819–4822CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Wallen K, Baum MJ (2002) Masculinization and defeminization in altricial and precocial mammals: comparative aspects of steroid hormone action. In: Pfaff DW (ed) Hormones, brain, and behavior. New York Academic Press, New York, pp 385–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Walters JR, Seyfarth RM (1987) Conflict and cooperation. In: Smutts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth MR, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 306–317Google Scholar
  80. Westergaard GC, Suomi SJ, Chavanne TJ, Houser L, Hurley A, Cleveland A, Snoy PJ, Higley JD (2003) Physiological correlates of aggression and impulsivity in free-ranging female primates. Neuropsychopharmacol 28:1045–1055Google Scholar
  81. Wilson JD, George FW, Griffin JE (1981) The hormonal control of sexual development. Science 211:1278–1284CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Zákány J, Frormental-Ramian C, Warot X, Duboule D (1997) Regulation of thenumber and size of the digits by posterior Hox genes: a dose-dependent mechanism with potential evolutionary implications. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:13695–13700CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Zehr JL, van Meter PE, Wallen K (2005) Factors regulating the timing of puberty onset in female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): role if prenatal androgens, social rank, and adolescent body weight. Biol Reprod 72:1087–1094CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Emma Nelson
    • 1
  • Christy L. Hoffman
    • 2
  • Melissa S. Gerald
    • 3
    • 4
  • Susanne Shultz
    • 5
  1. 1.School of Archaeology, Classics and EgyptologyUniversity of LiverpoolLiverpoolUK
  2. 2.Department of Comparative Human DevelopmentUniversity of ChicagoChicagoUSA
  3. 3.Department of Medicine, Medical Sciences CampusUniversity of Puerto RicoSan JuanUSA
  4. 4.Laboratory for Primate Morphology and GeneticsUniversity of Puerto Rico Medical SchoolSan JuanUSA
  5. 5.Institute of Cognitive and Evolutionary AnthropologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations