Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 64, Issue 2, pp 247–255

Sex differences in the movement patterns of free-ranging chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): foraging and border checking

Original Paper

Abstract

Most social primates live in cohesive groups, so travel paths inevitably reflect compromise: decision processes of individuals are obscured. The fission–fusion social organisation of the chimpanzee, however, allows an individual's movements to be investigated independently. We followed 15 chimpanzees (eight male and seven female) through the relatively flat forest of Budongo, Uganda, plotting the path of each individual over periods of 1–3 days. Chimpanzee movement was parsed into phases ending with halts of more than 20 min, during which individuals fed, rested or engaged in social activities. Males, lactating or pregnant females and sexually receptive females all travelled similar average distances between halts, at similar speeds and along similarly direct beeline paths. Compared to lactating or pregnant females, males did travel for a significantly longer time each day and halted more often, but the most striking sex differences appeared in the organisation of movement phases into a day's path. After a halt, males tended to continue in the same direction as before. Lactating or pregnant females showed no such strategy and often retraced the preceding phase, returning to previously visited food patches. We suggest that female chimpanzee movements approximate an optimal solution to feeding requirements, whereas the paths of males allow integration of foraging with territorial defence. The ‘continually moving forwards’ strategy of males enables them to monitor their territory boundaries—border checking—whilst foraging, generally avoiding the explicit boundary patrols observed at other chimpanzee study sites.

Keywords

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytesRange use Sex differences Fission–fusion Individual movement patterns Border checking 

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–265CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic, LondonGoogle Scholar
  3. Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Taï forest: behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  4. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW (1993) Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: implications for the understanding of chimpanzee social organisation. Am J Primatol 31:263–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chapman C, White F, Wrangham R (1994) Party size in chimpanzees and bonobos: A re-evaluation of theory based on two similarly forested sites. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 41–58Google Scholar
  6. Couzin I, Krausse J, Franks N, Levin S (2005) Effective leadership and decision making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433:513–516CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Croom Helm, LondonGoogle Scholar
  8. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  9. Janson CH, Byrne R (2007) What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box. Anim Cogn 10:357–367CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Krebs JR, Davies N (1997) Behavioural Ecology: an evolutionary approach, 4th edn. Blackwell Scientific, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  11. Kummer H (1968) Social organisation of Hamadryas baboons. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  12. Laughlin K, Mendl M (2004) Costs of acquiring and forgetting information affect spatial memory and its susceptibility to interference. Anim Behav 68:97–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lee PC (1987) Nutrition, fertility and maternal investment in primates. J Zool 213:409–422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lehmann J, Boesch C (2005) Bisexually bonded ranging in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:525–535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Moss CJ (1988) Elephant memories: thirteen years in the life of an elephant family. Elm Tree Books, LondonGoogle Scholar
  16. Newton-Fisher N (2000) Male chimpanzee core areas: ranging in Budongo Forest chimpanzees. Pan African News 7:10–12Google Scholar
  17. Newton-Fisher NE (2003) The home range of the Sonso community of chimpanzees from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Afr J Ecol 41:150–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Nishida T (1990) The chimpanzees of the Mahale mountains. Sexual and life-history strategies. University of Tokyo Press, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  19. Nishida T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M (1987) Chimpanzees and Bonobos: Cooperative relationships among males. In: Smuts B, Cheney D, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker T (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, London, pp 165–177Google Scholar
  20. Plumptre AJ, Cox D, Mugume S (2003) The status of chimpanzees in Uganda. Wildlife Conservation Society, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Prins HHT (1996) Ecology and behaviour of the African buffalo. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar
  22. Ramos-Fernandez G, Mateos J, Miramontes O, Larralde H, Ayala-Orozco B (2004) Levy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:223–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Reynolds V (2005) The chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Sherrow HM, Amsler SA (2007) New cases of intercommunity infanticides by the chimpanzees of Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Int J Primatol 28:9–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP (1997) The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:291–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Strier KB (2000) Primate behavioural ecology. Allyn & Bacon, BostonGoogle Scholar
  27. Tutin CEG (1979) Mating patterns and reproductive strategies in a community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 6:29–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. van Schaik CP (1989) The social ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Standen V, Foley RA (eds) Comparative socioecology. The behavioural ecology among humans and other animals. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 195–218Google Scholar
  29. Watts DP, Mitani JC (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:299–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Watts DP, Mitani JC, Sherrow HM (2002) New cases of inter-community infanticide by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. Primates 43(4):263–270CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. White FJ, Wrangham RW (1988) Feeding competition and patch size in the chimpanzee species Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes. Behaviour 105:148–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Williams JM, Pusey AE, Carlis JV, Farm BP, Goodall J (2002) Female competition and male territorial behaviour influence female chimpanzees ranging patterns. Anim Behav 63:347–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Williams JM, Oehlert GW, Carlis JV, Pusey AE (2004) Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range? Anim Behav 68:523–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wrangham R, Smuts B (1980) Sex differences in the behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Reproductive Fertility 28:13–31Google Scholar
  35. Wrangham RW (1979) Sex differences in chimpanzee dispersion. In: Hamburg DA, McCown E (eds) The great apes. Addison-Wesley, Menlo Park, pp 481–489Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of PsychologyUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsScotland

Personalised recommendations