Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 61, Issue 9, pp 1425–1436 | Cite as

Female and male serins (Serinus serinus) respond differently to derived song traits

  • Gonçalo C. Cardoso
  • Paulo Gama Mota
  • Violaine Depraz
Original Paper

Abstract

We tested if male or female behavior towards manipulated song indicates intra- or inter-sexual selection of two characteristics of serin song that are extreme and evolutionarily derived in this species: high frequency and fast syllable rate. In a first experiment, we monitored vocal responses and attendance to song playbacks. Female behavior indicated a preference for high-frequency song and suggested an aggressive function for fast syllable rates, as fast songs inhibited vocal response. Males did not show discrimination of frequency or syllable rate with this experimental design. The second experiment used a simple approach/no approach design, and in this experiment, males showed stronger discrimination between stimuli than did females. Therefore, sex differences in discrimination appear not to result from differences in perceptual abilities but from differences in the context of stimulus presentation. The second experiment also supported a role of song frequency in female choice, as the effect of frequency was limited to females: males did not respond differently to song frequency and approached high-frequency songs less than females did. Results of this experiment also supported an aggressive function for fast syllable rates, as the effect of fast songs did extend to male behavior. Taken together, our results indicate that the high frequency and fast syllable rate of serin song cannot result from a single selection process: while high frequency may have evolved by inter-sexual selection, syllable rate provokes a pattern of response that is more consistent with intra-sexual selection.

Keywords

Signal evolution Song frequency Syllable rate Intra-sexual selection Inter-sexual selection 

References

  1. Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  2. Andersson M, Norberg RÅ (1981) Evolution of reversed sexual size dimorphism and role partitioning among predatory birds, with a size scaling of flight performance. Biol J Linn Soc 15:105–130Google Scholar
  3. Anderson RC, Nowicki S, Searcy WA (2007) Soft song in song sparrows: response of males and females to an enigmatic signal. Behav Ecol Sociobiol (in press) DO 10.1007/s00265-007-0357-7
  4. Arnaiz-Villena A, Álvarez-Tejado M, Ruíz-del-Valle V, García-dela-Torre C, Varela P, Recio MJ, Ferre S, Martinez-Laso J (1999) Rapid radiation of canaries (genus Serinus). Mol Biol Evol 16:2–11Google Scholar
  5. Ballentine B, Hyman J, Nowick S (2004) Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav Ecol 15:163–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Becker PH (1982) The coding of species-specific characteristics in birds sounds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol. 1: production, perception and design features of sounds. Academic, New York, pp 214–252Google Scholar
  7. Berglund A, Bisazza A, Pilastro A (1996) Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biol J Linn Soc 58:385–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Blomqvist D, Johansson OC, Unger U, Larsson M, Flodin LA (1997) Male aerial display and reversed sexual size dimorphism in the dunlin. Anim Behav 54:1291–1299CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brown WD, Wideman J, Andrade MCB, Mason AC, Gwynne DT (1996) Female choice for an indicator of male size in the song of the black-horned tree cricket Oecanthus nigricornis (Orthoptera: Gryllidae: Oecanthinae). Evolution 50:2400–2411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Byers BE (2007) Extrapair paternity in chestnut-sided warblers is correlated with consistent vocal performance. Behav Ecol 18:130–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cardoso GC, Mota PG (2007) Song diversification and complexity in canaries and seedeaters (Serinus spp.). Biol J Linn Soc (in press)Google Scholar
  12. Catchpole CK (1987) Bird song, sexual selection and female choice. Trends Ecol Evol 2:94–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (1995) Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Christie PJ, Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM (2004) Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:341–348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Clayton NS (1988) Song discrimination in zebra finches. Anim Behav 36:1016–1024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Collins SA (2000) Men’s voices and women’s choices. Anim Behav 60:773–780CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Cramp S, Perrins CM (1994) The birds of the Western Paleartic, vol VIII. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Cynx J, Nottebohm F (1992) Role of gender, season, and familiarity in discrimination of conspecific song by zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:1368–1371PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB (1985) Correspondence between messages in the full song of the blackbird Turdus merula and meanings to territorial males, as inferred from responses to computerized modifications of natural song. Z Tierpsychol 69:149–165Google Scholar
  20. Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB (1993) Song-based discrimination and behaviour assessment by female blackbirds, Turdus merula. Anim Behav 45:759–771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Davies NB, Halliday TR (1978) Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274:683–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Diehl P, Helb H-W (1986) Radiotelemetric monitoring of heart-rate responses to song playback in blackbirds (Turdus merula). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:213–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Drǎgǎnoiu TI, Nagle L, Kreutzer M (2002) Directional female preference for an exaggerated trait in canary (Serinus canaria) song. Proc R Soc Biol Sci B 269:2525–2531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Enquist M, Arak A (1993) Selection of exaggerated male traits by female aesthetic senses. Nature 361:446–448PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Falls JB (1992) Playback: a historical perspective. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp 11–33Google Scholar
  26. Figuerola J (1999) A comparative study on the evolution of reversed size dimorphism in monogamous waders. Biol J Linn Soc 67:1–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Forstmeier W, Kempnaers B, Meyer A, Leisler B (2002) A novel song parameter correlates with extra-pair paternity and reflects male longevity. Proc R Soc Biol Sci B 269:1479–1485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gil D, Gahr M (2002) The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol Evol 17:133–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Grønstøl GB (1996) Aerobatic components in the song-flight display of male lapwings Vanellus vanellus as cues in female choice. Ardea 84:45–55Google Scholar
  30. Guilford T, Dawkins MS (1991) Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim Behav 42:1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Güttinger HR (1985) Consequences of domestication on the song structures in the canary. Behaviour 94:254–278Google Scholar
  32. Hakkarainen H, Hutta E, Lahti K, Lundvall P, Mappes T, Tolonen P, Wiehn J (1996) A test of male mating and hunting success in the kestrel: the advantages of smallness? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39:375–380CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hedenström A, Møller AP (1992) Morphological adaptations to song flight in passerine birds: a comparative study. Proc R Soc Biol Sci B 247:183–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Howard RD, Young JR (1998) Individual variation in male vocal traits and female mating preferences in Bufo americanus. Anim Behav 55:1165–1179CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hurly TA, Ratcliffe L, Weary DM, Weisman R (1992) White-throated sparrows (Zonotrichia albicollis) can perceive pitch change in conspecific song by using the frequency ratio independent of the frequency difference. J Comp Psychol 106:388–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ikebuchi M, Okanoya K (2000) Limited memory for conspecific songs in a non-territorial songbird. NeuroReport 11:3915–3919PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ikebuchi M, Futamatsu M, Okanoya K (2003) Sex differences in song perception in Bengalese finches measured by cardiac response. Anim Behav 65:123–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Illes AE, Hall ML, Vehrencamp SL (2006) Vocal performance influences male receiver responses in the banded wren. Proc R Soc Biol Sci B 273:1907–1912CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Johnsrude IS, Weary DM, Ratcliffe LM, Weisman RG (1994) Effect of motivational context on conspecific song discrimination by brown-headed cowbirds (Molothus ater). J Comp Psychol 108:172–178PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kingston JJ, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2003) The role of sexual selection in maintaining a colour polymorphism in the pigmy swordtail, Xiphophorus pygmaeus. Anim Behav 65:735–743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ladich F (1998) Sound characteristics and outcome of contests in male croaking gouramis (Teleostei). Ethology 104:517–529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lambrechts MM (1997) Song frequency plasticity and composition of phrase versions in great tits Parus major. Ardea 85:99–109Google Scholar
  43. Leitão A, Riebel K (2003) Are good ornaments bad armaments? Male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length. Anim Behav 66:161–167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mota PG (1999) The functions of song in the Serin. Ethology 105:137–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mota PG, Cardoso GC (2001) Song organisation and patterns of variation in the Serin (Serinus serinus). Acta Ethol 3:141–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mota PG, Depraz V (2004) A test of the effect of male song on female nesting behaviour in the serin (Serinus serinus): a field playback experiment. Ethology 110:841–850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nagle L, Kreutzer M, Vallet E (2002) Adult female canaries respond to male song by calling. Ethology 108:463–472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nelson DA (1989) The importance of invariant and distinctive features in species recognition of bird song. Condor 91:120–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Nelson DA, Soha JA (2004) Male and female white-crowned sparrows respond differently to geographic variation in song. Behaviour 141:53–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Nolan PM, Hill GE (2004) Female choice for song characteristics in the house finch. Anim Behav 67:403–410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Okanoya K, Dooling R (1987) Hearing in passerine and psittacine birds: a comparative study of absolute and masked auditory thresholds. J Comp Psychol 101:7–15PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Okanoya K, Dooling RJ, Downing JD (1990) Hearing and vocalisations in hybrid waterslager-roller canaries (Serinus canarius). Hear Res 46:271–276PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pacheco C (2002) Reconhecimento individual entre os membros do par e individualidade nas vocalizações de contacto na Milheirinha (Serinus serinus). M.Sc. Thesis, University of Coimbra, CoimbraGoogle Scholar
  54. Pfefferle D, Fischer J (2006) Sounds and size: identification of acoustic variables that reflect body size in hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas. Anim Behav 72:43–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Podos J (1997) A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51: 537–551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ratcliffe L, Otter K (1996) Sex differences in song recognition. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, pp 339–355Google Scholar
  57. Riebel K, Slater PJB (1998) Testing female chaffinch song preferences by operant conditioning. Anim Behav 56:1443–1453CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Riebel K, Smallegange IM, Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis J (2002) Sexual equality in zebra finch song preference: evidence for a dissociation between song recognition and production learning. Proc R Soc Biol Sci B 269:729–733CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Searcy WA, Andersson M (1986) Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Ann Rev Ecolog Syst 17:507–533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Suthers RA, Goller F, Pytte C (1999) The neuromuscular control of birdsong. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 354:927–939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tchernichovski O, Schwabl H, Nottebohm F (1998) Context determines the sex appeal of male zebra finch song. Anim Behav 55:1003–1010CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Vallet E, Kreutzer M (1995) Female canaries are sexually responsive to special song phrases. Anim Behav 49:1603–1610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Vallet E, Beme I, Kreutzer M (1998) Two-note syllables in canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Anim Behav 55:291–297CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Vehrencamp SL (2000) Handicap, index, and conventional signal elements of bird song. In: Espmark Y, Amundsen T, Rosenqvist G (eds) Animal signals: signalling and signal design in animal communication. Tapir Academic, Trondheim, pp 277–300Google Scholar
  65. Wallschläger D (1980) Correlation of song frequency and body weight in passerine birds. Experientia 36:412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wasserman FE, Cigliano JA (1991) Song output and stimulation of the female in white-throated sparrows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:55–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Zar JH (1996) Biostatistical analysis, 3rd edn. Prentice-Hall, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  68. Zimmer UE (1982) Birds react to playback of recorded songs by heart rate alteration. Z Tiërpsychol 58:25–30Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gonçalo C. Cardoso
    • 1
  • Paulo Gama Mota
    • 1
  • Violaine Depraz
    • 1
  1. 1.Departamento de AntropologiaFaculdade de Ciências e Tecnologia da Universidade de CoimbraCoimbraPortugal
  2. 2.Department of Ecology and EvolutionUniversity of ChicagoChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations