Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 61, Issue 5, pp 653–660 | Cite as

Orientation and navigation in bats: known unknowns or unknown unknowns?

Review

Abstract

Bats have been extensively studied with regard to their ability to orient, navigate and hunt prey by means of echolocation, but almost nothing is known about how they orient and navigate in situations such as migration and homing outside the range of their echolocation system. As volant animals, bats face many of the same problems and challenges as birds. Migrating bats must relocate summer and winter home ranges over distances as far as 2,000 km. Foraging bats must be able to relocate their home roost if they range beyond a familiar area, and indeed circumstantial evidence suggests that these animals can home from more than 600 km. However, an extensive research program on homing and navigation in bats halted in the early 1970s. The field of bird navigation has advanced greatly since that time and many of the mechanisms that birds are known to use for navigation were not known or widely accepted at this time. In this paper I discuss what is known about orientation and navigation in bats and use bird navigation as a model for future research in bat navigation. Technology is advancing such that previous difficulties in studying orientation in bats in the field can be overcome and so that the mechanisms of navigation in this highly mobile animal can finally be elucidated.

Keywords

Navigation Orientation Bats Map Compass 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Isabelle Bisson, Melissa Bowlin, Kasper Thorup and Martin Wikelski provided helpful comments on the manuscript. Support was from Marie Curie Outgoing International Fellowship MOIF-CT-2005-021508, administered by the University of Leeds.

References

  1. August PV, Ayvazian SG, Anderson JGT (1989) Magnetic orientation in a small mammal, Peromyscus leucopus. J Mamm 70:1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benvenuti S, Fiaschi V, Fiore L, Papi F (1973) Homing performances of inexperienced and directionally trained pigeons subjected to olfactory nerve section. J Comp Physiol 83:81–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berthold P (2001) Bird migration: a general survey, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  4. Bingman VP, Cheng K (2005) Mechanisms of animal global navigation: comparative perspectives and enduring challenges. Ethol Ecol Evol 17:295–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Buchler ER, Childs SB (1982) Use of the post-sunset glow as an orientation cue by big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). J Mammal 63:243–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buchler ER, Wasilewski PJ (1985) Magnetic remanence in bats. In: Kirschvink JL, Jones DS, MacFadden BJ (eds) Magnetite biomineralization and magnetoreception in organisms: a new biomagnetism. Plenum, New York, pp 483–488Google Scholar
  7. Chernetsov N, Berthold P, Querner U (2004) Migratory orientation of first-year white storks (Ciconia ciconia): inherited information and social interactions. J Exp Biol 207:937–943PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Childs SB, Buchler ER (1981) Perception of simulated stars by Eptesicus fuscus (Vespertilionidae)—a potential navigational mechanism. Anim Behav 29:1028–1035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cochran WW, Mouritsen H, Wikelski M (2004) Migrating songbirds recalibrate their magnetic compass daily from twilight cues. Science 304:405–408PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cryan PM (2003) Seasonal distribution of migratory tree bats (Lasiurus and Lasionycteris) in North America. J Mammal 84:579–593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cryan PM, Bogan MA, Rye RO, Landis GP, Kester CL (2004) Stable hydrogen isotope analysis of bat hair as evidence for seasonal molt and long-distance migration. J Mammal 85:995–1001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davis R (1966) Homing performance and homing ability in bats. Ecol Mono 36:201–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deutschlander ME, Freake MJ, Borland SC, Phillips JB, Madden RC, Anderson LE, Wilson BW (2003) Learned magnetic compass orientation by the Siberian hamster, Phodopus sungorus. Anim Behav 65:779–786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM, Phillips JB (2005) Magnetic compass mediates nocturnal homing by the alpine newt, Triturus alpestris. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58:361–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dwyer PD (1969) Population ranges of Miniopterus schreibersi (Chiroptera) in south-eastern Australia. Aust J Zool 17:665–686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Emlen ST (1967) Migratory orientation in Indigo Bunting Passerina Cyanea. I. Evidence for use of celestial cues. Auk 84:309–313Google Scholar
  17. Fischer JH, Freake MJ, Borland SC, Phillips JB (2001) Evidence for the use of magnetic map information by an amphibian. Anim Behav 62:1–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Flemming TH, Eby P (2003) Ecology of bat migration. In: Kunz TH, Fenton MB (eds) Bat ecology. University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, pp 156–208Google Scholar
  19. Freire R, Munro UH, Rogers LJ, Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (2005) Chickens orient using a magnetic compass. Curr Biol 15:R620–R621PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Griffin DR (1952) Bird navigation. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 27:359–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Griffin DR (1970) Migrations and homing of bats. In: Wimsatt WA (ed) Biology of bats, vol 1. Academic, New York, pp 233–265Google Scholar
  22. Hobson KA (1999) Tracing origins and migration of wildlife using stable isotopes: a review. Oecologia 120:314–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jouventin P, Weimerskirch H (1990) Satellite tracking of wandering albatrosses. Nature 343:746–748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kramer G (1953) Wird die Sonnenhöhe bei der Heimfindeorientierung verwertet? J Orn 94:201–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lohmann KJ, Lohmann CMF (1996) Orientation and open-sea navigation in sea turtles. J Exp Biol 199:73–81PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Lohmann KJ, Lohmann CMF, Ehrhart LM, Bagley DA, Swing T (2004) Animal behaviour—geomagnetic map used in sea-turtle navigation. Nature 428:909–910PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Marhold S, Wiltschko W, Burda H (1997) A magnetic polarity compass for direction finding in a subterranean mammal. Naturwissenschaften 84:421–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Merkel FW, Wiltschko W (1965) Magnetismus und Richtungsfinden zugunruhiger Rotkehlchen (Erithacus rubecula). Vogel 23:71–77Google Scholar
  29. Mora CV, Davison M, Wild JM, Walker MM (2004) Magnetoreception and its trigeminal mediation in the homing pigeon. Nature 432:508–511PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mouritsen H (2003) Spatiotemporal orientation strategies of long-distance migrants. In: Berthold P, Gwinner E, Sonnenschein E (eds) Avian migration. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 493–514Google Scholar
  31. Mueller HC (1963) Homing and distance orientation in bats. University of Wisconsin, MadisonGoogle Scholar
  32. Mueller HC, Emlen JT (1957) Homing in bats. Science 126:307–308CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Muheim R, Moore FR, Phillips JB (2006a) Calibration of magnetic and celestial compass cues in migratory birds—a review of cue-conflict experiments. J Exp Biol 209:2–17PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Muheim R, Phillips JB, Akesson S (2006b) Polarized light cues underlie compass calibration in migratory songbirds. Science 313:837–839PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mumford RE (1958) Population turnover in wintering bats in Indiana. J Mammal 39:253–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Munro U, Munro JA, Phillips JB, Wiltschko W (1997) Effect of wavelength of light and pulse magnetisation on different magnetoreception systems in a migratory bird. Aust J Zool 45:189–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Neuweiler G (2000) The biology of bats. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  38. Papi F (2001) Animal navigation at the end of the century: a retrospect and a look forward. Int J Zoonoses 68:171–180Google Scholar
  39. Parkyn DC, Austin JD, Hawryshyn CW (2003) Acquisition of polarized-light orientation in salmonids under laboratory conditions. Anim Behav 65:893–904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Perdeck AC (1958) Two types of orientation in migrating starlings, Sturnus vulgaris L. and chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs L., as revealed by displacement experiments. Ardea 46:1–37Google Scholar
  41. Phillips JB (1996) Magnetic navigation. J Theor Biol 180:309–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pimm SL, Jones HL, Diamond J (1988) On the risk of extinction. Am Nat 132:757–785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Racey PA, Entwistle AC (2003) Conservation ecology of bats. In: Kunz TH, Fenton MB (eds) Bat ecology. University of Chicago, Chicago, pp 680–744Google Scholar
  44. Serra-Cobo J, Lopez-Roig M, Marques-Bonet T, Lahuerta E (2000) Rivers as possible landmarks in the orientation flight of Miniopterus schreibersii. Acta Theriol 45:347–352Google Scholar
  45. Smith E, Goodpaster W (1958) Homing in nonmigratory bats. Science 127:644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Stones RC, Branick LP (1969) Use of hearing in homing by 2 species of Myotis bats. J Mammal 50:157–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Thorup K (2003) The migratory orientation programme in birds. In: Zoological Museum, Faculty of Science. University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, pp 171Google Scholar
  48. Twente JW (1955) Some aspects of habitat selection and other behavior of cavern-dwelling bats. Ecology 36:706–732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Walcott C (1991) Magnetic maps in pigeons. In: Berthold P (ed) Orientation in birds. Birkhauser Verlag, Berlin, pp 38–51Google Scholar
  50. Walcott C (2005) Multi-modal orientation cues in homing pigeons. Int Comp Biol 45:574–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Walker MM (1998) On a wing and a vector: a model for magnetic navigation by homing pigeons. J Theor Biol 192:341–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wallraff HG (1991) Conceptual approaches to avian navigation. In: Berthold P (ed) Orientation in birds. Birkhäuser Verlag, Berlin, pp 16–37Google Scholar
  53. Wallraff HG (1996) Seven theses on pigeon homing deduced from empirical findings. J Exp Biol 199:105–111PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Wallraff HG (1999) The magnetic map of homing pigeons: an evergreen phantom. J Theor Biol 197:265–269PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wallraff HG (2005) Avian navigation: pigeon homing as a paradigm. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  56. Wallraff HG, Kiepenheuer J (1994) Relationships between initial orientation and subsequent homing of individual pigeons. Anim Behav 47:821–832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wehner R (1998) Navigation in context: grand theories and basic mechanisms. J Avian Biol 29:370–386Google Scholar
  58. Williams TC, Williams JM (1967) Radio tracking of homing bats. Science 155:1435–1436PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Williams TC, Williams JM (1970) Radio tracking of homing and feeding flights of a neotropical bat, Phyllostomus hastatus. Anim Behav 18:302–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Williams TC, Williams JM, Griffin DR (1966a) Homing ability of neotropical bat Phyllostomus hastatus with evidence for visual orientation. Anim Behav 14:468–473PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Williams TC, Williams JM, Griffin DR (1966b) Visual orientation in homing bats. Science 152:677–679Google Scholar
  62. Willis CKR, Brigham RM (2004) Roost switching, roost sharing and social cohesion: forest-dwelling big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, conform to the fission-fusion model. Anim Behav 68:495–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wilson DE (1988) Maintaining bats for captive study. In: Kunz TH (ed) Ecological and behavioral methods for the study of bats. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC, pp 247–263Google Scholar
  64. Wiltschko R (1996) The function of olfactory input in pigeon orientation: Does it provide navigational information or play another role? J Exp Biol 199:113–119PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (1978) Evidence for use of magnetic outward-journey information in homing pigeons. Naturwissenschaften 65:112–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (2002) Magnetic compass orientation in birds and its physiological basis. Naturwissenschaften 89:445–452PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (2003) Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts. Anim Behav 65:257–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (2005) Magnetic orientation and magnetoreception in birds and other animals. J Comp Physiol A 191:675–693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Zimmerman PH, Pope SJ, Guilford T, Nicol CJ (2003) Navigational ability in the domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus). Appl Anim Behav Sci 80:327–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Zinn TL, Baker WW (1979) Seasonal migration of the hoary bat, Lasiurus cinereus, through Florida. J Mammal 60:634–635CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ecology and Evolutionary Biology DepartmentPrinceton UniversityPrincetonUSA
  2. 2.Institute of Integrative and Comparative BiologyUniversity of LeedsLeedsUK

Personalised recommendations