Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 60, Issue 2, pp 260–269 | Cite as

Heterospecific song matching in two closely related songbirds (Parus major and P. caeruleus): great tits match blue tits but not vice versa

Original Article

Abstract

To date, song research has focused primarily on the interactions of conspecifics. However, frequent interactions of songbirds with heterospecifics may necessitate adequate communication outside the species boundary. In this study, we focus on heterospecific communication behaviour of two small sympatric congeneric passerines, great and blue tits (Parus major and Parus caeruleus), which breed in overlapping territories and compete for food and nesting cavities. By means of a first playback experiment, we show that (1) heterospecific matching (imitating songs of the other species) is a strategy frequently used by great tits but not by blue tits, (2) both blue tit trilled and untrilled song can be accurately matched by great tits and that (3) almost half of the great tits in our study population match at least one blue tit song across all studied breeding stages, indicating that this heterospecific matching behaviour is a common feature in this population. A second playback experiment showed that these great tit imitations of blue tit songs do not function in intraspecific communication between male great tits. Hence, these heterospecific imitations appear to be designed for interspecific communication with blue tits. These findings suggest a strong heterospecific influence on the vocal learning process, repertoire composition and repertoire use in great tits and provide a possible mechanism that can drive song convergence in songbirds.

Keywords

Heterospecific matching Interspecies communication Vocal imitations 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to R. Oblonsek for the help during fieldwork and the time consuming scoring of sonograms, P. Scheys for help during fieldwork and T. Snoeijs for statistical advice. L. Gorissen was funded by a Ph.D. grant of the Institute for the Promotion of Innovation through Science and Technology in Flanders (IWT Vlaanderen). All experiments performed comply with the current laws of the Belgian Government.

References

  1. Baptista LF, Catchpole CK (1989) Vocal mimicry and interspecific aggression in songbirds: experiments using white-crowned sparrow imitation of song sparrow song. Behaviour 109:247–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baylis JR (1982) Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds. Production, perception and design features of sounds. Academic, New York pp 51–83Google Scholar
  3. Becker PH (1982) The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds. Production, perception and design features of sounds. Academic, New York pp 213–252Google Scholar
  4. Beecher MD, Campbell SE, Burt JM, Hill CE, Nordby JC (2000) Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim Behav 59:21–27PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blakey JK (1994) Genetic evidence for extra-pair fertilizations in a monogamous passerine, the great Parus major. Ibis 136:457–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brémond JC (1968) Recherches sur la sémantique et les elements vecteurs d’information dans les signaux acoustique du rouge-gorge (Erithacus rubecula L.). Terre Vie 2:109–220Google Scholar
  7. Catchpole CK (1978) Interspecific territorialism and competition Acrocephalus warblers as revealed by playback experiments in area of sympatry and allopatry. Anim Behav 26:1072–1082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Catchpole CK, Baptista LF (1988) A test of the competition hypothesis of vocal mimicry, using song sparrow imitation of white-crowned sparrow song. Behaviour 106:119–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (1995) Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge Univ Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  10. Cody ML (1973) Character convergence. Annu Rev Syst Ecol 4:189–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cramp S, Perrins CM (1993) The birds of the Western Paleartic, vol. VII. Oxford Univ Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  12. Dabelsteen T, McGregor PK, Shepherd M, Whittaker X, Pedersen SB (1996) Is the signal value of overlapping different from that of alternating during matched singing in Great Tits? J Avian Biol 27:189–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dabelsteen T, McGregor PK, Holland J, Tobias JA, Pedersen SB (1997) The signal function of overlapping singing in male robins. Anim Behav 53:249–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dobkin DS (1979) Functional and evolutionary relationships of vocal copying phenomena in birds. Z Tierpsychol 50: 348–363Google Scholar
  15. Dhondt AA (1977) Interspecific competition between great and blue tit. Nature 268:521–523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dhondt AA, Eyckerman R (1980) Competition and the regulation of numbers in great and blue tit. Ardea 68:121–132Google Scholar
  17. Doutrelant C, Lambrechts MM (2001) Macrogeographic variation in song—a test of competition and habitat effects in blue tits. Ethology 107:533–544CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Doutrelant C, Aubin T, Hitier S, Lambrechts MM (1998) Two distinct song populations of blue tits, Parus caeruleus, in the French Mediterranean. Bioacoustics 9:1–16Google Scholar
  19. Doutrelant C, Leitao A, Otter K, Lambrechts MM (2000) Effect of blue tit song syntax on great tit territorial responsiveness—an experimental test of the character shift hypothesis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:119–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Falls JB, Krebs JR, McGregor PK (1982) Song matching in the great tit (Parus major): the effect of similarity and familiarity. Anim Behav 30:997–1009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ferguson JWH, van Zyl A, Delport K (2002) Vocal mimicry in African ossypha robin chats. J Ornithol 139:319–330Google Scholar
  22. Geberzahn N, Hultsch H (2003) Long-time storage of song types in birds: evidence from interactive playbacks. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:1085–1090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Geberzahn N, Hultsch H, Todt D (2002) Latent song type memories are accessible through auditory stimulation in a hand-reared songbird. Anim Behav 64:783–790CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gorissen M (2002) Onderzoek naar de akoestische interacties tussen pimpelmezen en koolmezen. Master’s Thesis, University of Antwerp, AntwerpGoogle Scholar
  25. Gorissen L (2005) The vocal communication strategies of blue and great tits, studied from an intra- and interspecific perspective. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Antwerp, AntwerpGoogle Scholar
  26. Gorissen L, Eens M (2004) Interactive communication between male and female great tits (Parus major) during the dawn chorus. Auk 121:184–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Haavie J, Borge T, Bures S, Garamszegi LZ, Lampe HM, Moreno J, Qvarnström A, Török J, Saetre G-P (2004) Flycatcher song in allopatry and sympatry—convergence, divergence and reinforcement. J Evol Biol 17:227–237PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hailman JP (1977) Optical signals. Indiana Univ Press, BloomingtonGoogle Scholar
  29. Hough GE II, Nelson DA, Volman SF (2000) Re-expression of songs deleted during vocal development in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys. Anim Behav 60:279–287PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Jones AE, Ten Cate C, Bijleveld CCJH (2001) The interobserver reliability of scoring sonograms by eye: a study on methods, illustrated on zebra finch songs. Anim Behav 62:791–801CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kempenaers B, Dhondt AA (1991) Competition between blue and great tit for roosting sites in winter: an aviary experiment. Ornis Scand 22:73–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kostan KM (2002) The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. J Comp Psychol 116:206–209PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Krebs JR, Avery M, Cowie R (1981a) Effect of mate removal on singing behaviour of great tits. Anim Behav 29:635–637CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Krebs JR, Ashcroft R, van Orsdol K (1981b) Song matching in the great tit (Parus major L.). Anim Behav 29:918–923CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Krokene C, Rigstad K, Dale M, Lifjeld JT (1998) The function of extrapair paternity in blue tits and great tits: good genes or fertility assurance? Behav Ecol 9:649–656CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kroodsma DE, Byers, BE, Goodale E, Johnson S, Liu W-C (2001) Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Anim Behav 61:1029–1033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lambrechts MM (1996) Organisation of birdsong and constrains on performance. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca pp 305-320Google Scholar
  38. Lubjuhn T, Strohbach S, Brün J, Gerken T, Epplen JT (1999) Extra-pair paternity in great tits (Parus major)—a long term study. Behaviour 136:1157–1172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Martin PR, Martin TE (2001) Behavioral interactions between coexisting species: song playback experiments with wood warblers. Ecology 82:207–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Martin PR, Fotheringham JR, Ratcliffe L, Robertson RJ (1996) Response of American redstarts (Suborder Passeri) and least flycatcher (Suborder Tyrannii) to heterospecific playback: the role of song in aggressive interactions and interference competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39:227–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. McGregor PK, Avery MI (1986) The unsung songs of great tits (Parus major): learning neighbours’ songs for discrimination. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:311–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. McGregor PK, Krebs JR (1989) Song learning in adult great tits tits (Parus major): effects of neighbours. Behaviour 108:139–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. McGregor PK, Dabelsteen T, Shepherd M, Pedersen S (1992) The signal value of matched singing in great tits: evidence from interactive playback experiments. Anim Behav 43:987–998Google Scholar
  44. Minot EO (1981) Effects of interspecific competition for food in breeding blue and great tits. J Anim Ecol 50:375–385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Molles LE, Vehrencamp SV (2001) Neighbour recognition by resident males in the banded wren, Thryothorus pleurostictus, a tropical songbird with high song type sharing. Anim Behav 61:119–127PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Morton ES (1976) Vocal mimicry in the thick-billed Euphonia. Willson Bull 88:485–487Google Scholar
  47. Nelson DA (2000) Song overproduction, selective attrition and song dialects in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim Behav 60:887–898PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nowicki S, Westneat MW, Hoese WJ (1992) Birdsong: motor function and the evolution of communication. Semin neurosci 4:385–390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Osiejuk TS, Kuczynski L (2000) Mixed and atypical singers among treecreepers Certhia brachydactyla and C. familiaris: a review and preliminary data from western Poland. Biol Bull Pozn 37:83–94Google Scholar
  50. Osiejuk TS, Kuczynski L (2003) Response to typical, mixed and shortened song versions in Eurasian treecreepers, Certhia familiaris. Biologia Bratislava 58:985–989Google Scholar
  51. Owen-Ashley NT, Schoech SJ, Mumme RL (2002) Context-specific response of Florida scrub-jay pairs to northern mockingbird vocal mimicry. Condor 104:858–865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Perrins CM (1979) British Tits. Collins, LondonGoogle Scholar
  53. Poesel A, Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB (2004) Dawn song of male blue tits as a predictor of competitiveness in midmorning singing interactions. Acta Ethol 6:65–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Reed TM (1982) Interspecific territoriality in the chaffinch and great tit on islands and the mainland of Scotland: playback and removal experiments. Anim Behav 30:171–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ryan MJ, Brenowitz EA (1985) The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126:87–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Robinson SK, Terborgh J (1995) Interspecific aggression and habitat selection by Amazonian birds. J Anim Ecol 64:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Schottler B (1995) Songs of blue tits Parus caeruleus palmensis from La Palma (Canary Islands)—a test of hypotheses. Bioacoustics 6:135–152Google Scholar
  58. Secondi J, Bretagnolle V, Compagnon C, Faivre B (2003) Species-specific song convergence in a moving hybrid zone between two passerines. Biol J Linn Soc 80:507–517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Slikas B, Sheldon FH, Gill FB (1996) Phylogeny of titmice (Paridae): estimate of relationships among subgenera based on DNA–DNA hybridization. J Avian Biol 27:70–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sorjonen J (1986) Mixed singing and interspecific territoriality—consequences of secondary contact of two ecologically and morphologically similar nightingale species in Europe. Ornis Scand 17:53–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Strohbach S, Curio E, Bathen A, Epplen JT, Lubjuhn T (1998) Extra-pair paternity in the great tit (Parus major): a test of the good genes hypothesis. Behav Ecol 9:388–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Todt D, Naguib M (2000) Vocal interactions in birds: the use of song as a model in communication. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt JS, Snowdon CT, Roper TJ (eds) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 29. Academic, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  63. Törok J, Toth L (1999) Asymmetric competition between two tit species: a reciprocal removal experiment. J Anim Ecol 68:338–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Biology Campus Drie Eiken,University of AntwerpWilrijkBelgium

Personalised recommendations