Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 58, Issue 5, pp 437–445 | Cite as

The neglected scent: on the marking function of urine in Lemur catta

  • E. Palagi
  • L. Dapporto
  • S. Borgognini Tarli
Original Article

Abstract

In ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) urine deposition can be combined with two different tail configurations: (i) tail held up in an evident display (urinate tail-up, UT-up); (ii) tail slightly raised to avoid its impregnation with urine (urinate tail-down, UT-down). We used both signaller- and receiver-based approaches to search for functional differences between these two kinds of urine deposition. We predicted that UT-up might be a complex signal combining olfactory and visual cues. We carried out observations and scent tests on four captive groups of ring-tailed lemurs. Group members sniffed/licked UT-up scents more frequently than UT-down ones. Moreover, UT-up showed peak levels during the mating season whereas UT-down did not. These findings suggest that urine can play a role in intra-group reproductive communication. Lemurs more frequently performed UT-up in a few drops and UT-down in streams. Recognition experiments clearly showed that individuals can discriminate between urine of their own group and urine from a foreign group (a necessary prerequisite for the use of urine in inter-group communication). The possible function of UT-up in inter-troop communication was supported by the higher frequency of this pattern along a fence separating two of the study groups. Moreover, in the presence of a dummy, the frequency of UT-up increased significantly. In conclusion, UT-up is a complex signal with multiple characteristics. By using different sensory channels, UT-up provides different types of information (location and signaller quality) and contains multiple messages directed both at group-members and neighbouring groups.

Keywords

Complex signal Inter-group communication Intra-group communication Lemur catta Urine marking 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thanks are due to Paolo Cavicchio (Giardino Zoologico Città di Pistoia, Pistoia, Italy), Iole Palanca, Renato Piccinini (Parco Zoo di Falconara, Falconara Marittima, AN, Italy), Maria Rodeano (Parco Zoo Punta Verde, Lignano Sabbiadoro, UD, Italy) for allowing and facilitating this work, Ruth Curreli, Sabrina Telara and Alessandro Pardini for helping with data collection, Eckhard Heymann and Anne Mertl for their critical revision, Peter Kappeler and two anonymous referees for improving the manuscript, Tommaso Paoli and Ivan Norscia for useful suggestions, and to Isabel Behncke for language revision. This research was funded by Giardino Zoologico Città di Pistoia, Parco Zoo di Falconara, Parco Zoo Punta Verde, and the University of Pisa. All the experimental procedures conformed to Italian law

References

  1. Albone ES (1984) Mammalian semiochemistry: the investigation of chemical signals between mammals. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Altman J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Begg CM, Begg KS, Du Toit JT, Mills MGL (2003) Scent-marking behaviour of the honey badger, Mellivora capensis (Mustelidae), in the southern Kalahari. Anim Behav 66:917–929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Biggins JG (1984) Communication in possums: A review. In: Smith AP, Hume ID (eds) Possums and gliders. Australian Mammal Society, Sydney, pp 35–57Google Scholar
  5. Box HO (1977) Quantitative data on the carrying of young captive monkey (Callithrix jacchus) by other members of their family groups. Primates 18:475–484Google Scholar
  6. Bradbury JK, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer Associates, SunderlandGoogle Scholar
  7. Brockman DK, Whitten PL (1996) Reproduction in free-ranging Propithecus verrauxi: Estrus and the relationship between multiple partner matings and fertilization. Am J Phys Anthropol 100:57–69CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown RE, MacDonald DW (1985) Social odours in mammals, vol. 2. Clarendon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  9. Buesching CD, Macdonald DW (2001) Scent-marking behaviour of the European badger (Meles meles): Resource defence or individual advertisement? In: Marchlewska-Koj J, Lepri J, Müller-Schwarze D (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates. Plenum Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  10. Buesching CD, Newman C, Macdonald DW (2002a) Variations in colour and volume of the subcaudal gland secretion of badgers (Meles meles) in relation to sex, season and individual-specific parameters. Mammal Biol 67:147–156Google Scholar
  11. Buesching CD, Waterhouse JS, Macdonald DW (2002b) Gas-chromatographic analysis of the subcaudal gland secretion of the European badger (Meles meles) part II: Time-related variation in the individual-specific composition. J Chem Ecol 28:57–69CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Burt WH (1943) Territoriality and home range concepts applied to mammals. J Mammal 24:346–352Google Scholar
  13. Chandler CF (1975) Development and function of marking and sexual behaviour in the Malagasy prosimian primate, Lemur fulvus. Primates 16:35–47Google Scholar
  14. Curtis DJ, Zaramody A, Green DI, Pickard AR (2000) Non-invasive monitoring of reproductive status in wild mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz). Repr Fert Dev 12:21–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dawkins MS, Guilford T (1994) Design of an intention signal in the bluehead wrasse (Thalassoma bifasciatum). Proc R Soc Lond B 257:123–128Google Scholar
  16. Desjardins C, Maruniak JA, Bronson FH (1973) Social rank in house mice: differentiation revealed by ultraviolet visualization of urinary marking patterns. Science 182:939–941PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Devenport L, Devenport J, Kokesh C (1999) The role of urine marking in the foraging behaviour of least chipmunks. Anim Behav 57:557–563CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Dugmore SJ, Bailey K, Evans C (1984) Discrimination by male ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) between the scent marks of male and female conspecifics. Int J Primatol 5:235–245Google Scholar
  19. Epple G (1970) Quantitative studies on scent marking in the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus jacchus). Folia Primatol 13:48–62PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Epple G (1986) Communication by chemical signals. In: Mitchell G, Erwin J (eds) Comparative primate biology 2A. Liss, New York, pp 531–580Google Scholar
  21. Estes RD (1969) Territorial behavior of the wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus Burchell 1832). Z Tierpsychol 26:284–370Google Scholar
  22. Evans CS, Goy RW (1968) Social behaviour and reproductive cycles in captive ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). J Zool 156:181–197Google Scholar
  23. Fisher HS, Swaisgood RR, Fitch-Snyder H (2003) Countermarking by male pygmy lorises (Nycticebus pygmaeus): do females use odour cues to select mates with high competitive ability? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:123–130Google Scholar
  24. Fornasieri I, Roeder JJ (1992) Marking behaviour in two lemur species (L. fulvus and L. macaco): relation to social status, reproduction, aggression and environmental change. Folia Primatol 59:137–148PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. French JA, Cleveland J (1984). Scent-marking in the tamarin, Saguinus oedipus: Sex differences and ontogeny. Anim Behav 32:615–623Google Scholar
  26. Gosling LM, (1986) Economic consequences of scent marking in mammalian territoriality. In: Duvall D, Müller-Schwarze D, Silverstein RM (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates 4. Plenum Press, New York, pp 385–395Google Scholar
  27. Gosling LM, (1990) Scent-marking by resource holders: alternative mechanisms for advertising the costs of competition. In: MacDonald DW, Muller- Schwarze D, Natynczuk E (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates 5. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 315–328Google Scholar
  28. Gosling LM, Roberts SC (2001). Scent-marking by male mammals: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates. Adv Stud Behav 30:169–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gosling LM, Wright KHM (1994) Scent marking and resource defence by male coypus (Myocastor coypus). J Zool 234:423–436Google Scholar
  30. Guilford T, Dawkins MS (1991) Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim Behav 42:1–14Google Scholar
  31. Harrington JE (1976) Discrimination between individuals by scent in Lemur fulvus. Anim Behav 24:207–212PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Harrington JE (1979) Responses of Lemur fulvus to scents of different subspecies of L. fulvus and to scents of different species of lemuriformes. Z Tierpsychol 49:1–9Google Scholar
  33. Hebets A, Papaj DR (2005) Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:197–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Henry JD (1977) The use of urine marking in the scavenging behaviour of the red fox (Vulpes vulpes). Behaviour 62:82–105Google Scholar
  35. Hurst JL (1989) The complex network of olfactory communication in populations of wild house mice Mus musculus Rutty: urine marking and investigation within family groups. Anim Behav 37:705–725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hurst JL (1990) Urine marking in populations of wild house mice Mus musculus Rutty. I. Communication between males. Anim Behav 40:209–222Google Scholar
  37. Irwin MT, Samonds KE, Raharison J, Write PC (2004) Lemur latrines: observations of latrine behaviour in wild primates and possible ecological significance. J Mammal 85:420–427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Johnson RP (1973) Scent marking in mammals. Anim Behav 21:521–535Google Scholar
  39. Johnstone RA (1996) Multiple displays in animal communication: “backup signals” and “multiple messages”. Philos T Roy Soc B 351:329–338Google Scholar
  40. Jolly A (1966) Lemur behaviour: a madagascar field study. The University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  41. Jolly A. (1967) Breeding synchrony in wild Lemur catta. In: Altmann SA (eds) Social communication among primates. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 3–13Google Scholar
  42. Jolly A, Oliver WLR, O’Connor SM (1982) Population and troop ranges of Lemur catta and Lemur fulvus at Berenty, Madagascar: 1980 census. Folia Primatol 39:115–123PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Jolly A, Rasamimanana HR, Kinnarid MF, O’Brien TG, Crowley HM, Harcourt CS, Gardner S, Davidson JM (1993) Territoriality in Lemur catta groups during the birth season at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Kappeler PM, Ganzhorn J (eds) Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Plenum Press, New York, London, pp 85–109Google Scholar
  44. Kappeler PM (1998) To whom it may concern: the transmission and function of chemical signals in Lemur catta. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:411–421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kruuk H (1978) Social organization and social behaviour of the European badger Meles meles. J Zool 185:205–212Google Scholar
  46. Kruuk H, Gorman M, Leitch A (1984) Scent-marking with the subcaudal gland by the European badger, Meles meles. Anim Behav 32:899–907Google Scholar
  47. MacDonald DW (1985) The carnivores: Order Carnivora. In: Brown RE, MacDonald DW (eds) Social odours in mammals. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 619–722Google Scholar
  48. Markl H (1983) Vibrational communication. In: Huber R, Markl H (eds) Neuroethology and behavioral physiology. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 332–353Google Scholar
  49. Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  50. Mertl AS (1975) Discrimination of individuals by scent in a primate. Behav Ecol 14:505–509Google Scholar
  51. Mertl AS (1976) Olfactory and visul cues in social interactions of Lemur catta. Folia Primatol 26:151–161PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Mertl-Millhollen AS (1986) Territorial scent marking by two sympatric lemur species. In: Duvall D, Müller-Schwarze D, Silverstein RM (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates 4. Plenum Press, New York, pp 385–395Google Scholar
  53. Mertl-Millhollen AS (1988) Olfactory demarcation of territorial but not home range boundaries by Lemur catta. Folia Primatol 50:175–187PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Mert-Millhollen AS, Gustafson HL, Budnitz N, Dainis K, Jolly A (1979) Population and territory stability of the Lemur catta at Berenty, Madagascar. Folia Primatol 31:106–122PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Mugford RA, Nowell NW (1971) The preputial gland as a source of aggression promoting odour in mice. Physiol Behav 6:247–249CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Mundry R, Fischer J (1998) Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often lead to incorrect P values: examples from Animal Behaviour. Anim Behav 56:256–259CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Nievergelt CM, Mutschler T, Feistner ATC (1998) Group encounters and territoriality in wild Alaotran gentle lemurs (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis). Am J Primatol 46:251–258CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Palagi E, Telara S, Borgognini Tarli SM (2003) Sniffing behaviour in Lemur catta: seasonality, sex, and rank. Int J Primatol 24:335–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Palagi E, Telara S, Borgognini Tarli SM (2004) Reproductive strategies in Lemur catta: the balance among sending, receiving, and counter-marking scent signals. Int J Primatol 25:1019–1031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Pereira ME, Kappeler PM (1997) Divergent systems of agonistic behaviour in lemurid primates. Behaviour 134:225–274Google Scholar
  61. Ramsay NF, Giller PS (1996) Scent-marking in ring-tailed lemurs: responses to the introduction of “foreign” scent in the home range. Primates 37:13–23Google Scholar
  62. Raymer JD, Weisler D, Novotny M, Asa C, Seal US, Mech LD (1983) Volatile constituents of wolf (Canis lupus) urine as related to gender and season. Experientia 40:707–709Google Scholar
  63. Rosell F, Bergan F, Parker H (1998) Scent-marking in the Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber) as a means of territory defence. J Chem Ecol 24:207–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rowe C (1999) Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals. Anim Behav 58:921–931PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Rowe C, Guilford T (1999) Novelty in a multimodal warning signal. Anim Behav 57:341–346CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Sauther ML (1991) Reproductive behaviour of free-ranging Lemur catta at Beza Mahafaly special reserve, Madagascar. Am J Phys Anthropol 84:463–477Google Scholar
  67. Sauther ML, Sussman RW, Gould L (1999) The socioecology of the ring-tailed lemur: Thirty-five years of research. Evol Anthropol 8:120–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schilling A (1979) Olfactory communication in prosimians. In: Doyle GA, Martin RD (eds) The study of prosimian behaviour. Academic press, New York, pp 461–542Google Scholar
  69. Siegel S, Castellan NJ Jr (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioural sciences. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  70. Sussman RW (1974) Ecological distinctions in sympatric species of Lemur. In: Martin RD, Doyle GA, Walker AC (eds) Prosimians biology. Gerald Duckworth, London, pp 75–108Google Scholar
  71. Van Horn RN (1975) Primate breeding season: photoperiodic regulation in Lemur catta. Folia Primatol 24:203–220PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Van Horn RN (1980) Seasonal reproductive patterns in primates. Prog Reprod Biol 5:181–221Google Scholar
  73. Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Belknap University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  74. Zub K, Theuerkauf J, Jedrzejewski W, Jedrzejewska B, Schmidt K, Kowalczyk R (2003) Wolf pack territory marking in the Bialowieza primeval forest (Poland). Behaviour 140:635–648CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centro Interdipartimentale Museo di Storia Naturale e del TerritorioUniversità di PisaCalci, PisaItaly
  2. 2.Dipartimento di Etologia, Ecologia ed EvoluzioneUnità di Antropologia, Università di Pisa 11Calci, PisaItaly

Personalised recommendations