Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 59, Issue 5, pp 644–650 | Cite as

Social structure and co-operative interactions in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata)

  • D. P. CroftEmail author
  • R. James
  • P. O. R. Thomas
  • C. Hathaway
  • D. Mawdsley
  • K. N. Laland
  • J. Krause
Original Article

Abstract

In contrast to the substantial number of theoretical papers that have examined the mechanisms by which cooperation may evolve, very few studies have investigated patterns of co-operation in natural animal populations. In the current study, we use a novel approach, social network analysis, to investigate the structure of co-operative interactions in the context of predator inspection in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Female guppies showed social preferences for stable partners, fulfilling a key assumption made by models of reciprocity. In the laboratory, wild female guppies disproportionately engaged in predator inspection with others with whom they had strong social associations. Furthermore, pairs of fish that frequently engaged in predator inspection did so in a particularly co-operative way, potentially reducing costs associated with predator inspection. Taken together, these results provide evidence for assortative interactions forming the basis of co-operation during predator inspection in a natural fish population. The occurrence of highly interconnected social networks between stable partners suggests the existence of co-operation networks in free-ranging populations of the guppy.

Keywords

Social networks Shoaling Tit-for-tat 

Notes

Acknowledgements

DPC would like to acknowledge funding from the Leverhulme Trust and the FSBI. We would also like to thank Ronnie Hernandez and the Board of Asa Wright Nature Centre, for their continued support in Triniwdad, Jenny James and Marc Botham for assistance in the field, Manfred Milinski, Jean-Guy Godin and Iain Couzin for insightful discussion and comments.

References

  1. Allee WC (1951) Cooperation among animals. Schuman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Boesch C (1994) Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 48:653–667CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Freeman LC (2002) Ucinet for Windows: Software for social network analysis. Analytic Technologies, HarvardGoogle Scholar
  4. Croft DP, Albanese B, Arrowsmith BJ, Botham M, Webster M, Krause J (2003a) Sex biased movement in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Oecologia 137:62–68PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Croft DP, Arrowsmith BJ, Bielby J, Skinner K, White E, Couzin ID, Magurran AE, Ramnarine I, Krause J (2003b) Mechanisms underlying shoal composition in the Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Oikos 100:429–438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Croft DP, Botham MS, Krause J (2004a) Is sexual segregation in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata, consistent with the predation risk hypothesis? Environ Biol Fisches 71:127–133Google Scholar
  7. Croft DP, Krause J, James R (2004b) Social networks in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Proc R Soc Lond Biol Lett 271:516–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Croft DP, James R, Ward AJW, Botham MS, Mawdsley D, Krause J (2005) Assortative interactions and social networks in fish. Oecologia 143:211–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Crowley PH (1992) Resampling methods for computation-intensive data-analysis in ecology and evolution. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 23:405–477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Darwin C (1859) The origin of species. J. Murray, LondonGoogle Scholar
  11. Dugatkin LA (1988) Do guppies play tit for tat during predator inspection visits. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:395–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dugatkin LA (1992) Tendency to inspect predators predicts mortality risk in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol 3:124–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dugatkin LA (1997) Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Dugatkin LA, Alfieri M (1991) Guppies and the tit-for-tat strategy — preference based on past interaction. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28:243–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dugatkin LA, Godin JGJ (1992) Predator inspection, shoaling and foraging under predation hazard in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Environ Biol Fisches 34:265–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dugatkin LA, Wilson DS (2000) Assortative interactions and the evolution of cooperation during predator inspection in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Evol Ecol Res 2:761–767Google Scholar
  17. Gatewood JB (1984) Cooperation, competition, and synergy — information-sharing groups among southeast Alaskan salmon seiners. Am Ethnol 11:350–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Griffiths SW (2003) Learned recognition of conspecifics by fishes. In: Fish are smarter than you think: learning in fishes, Brown C, Laland KN, Krause J (eds) Fish Fish 4:256–268 (special edition)Google Scholar
  19. Griffiths SW, Magurran AE (1998) Sex and schooling behaviour in the Trinidadian guppy. Anim Behav 56:689–693PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Haccou P, Meelis E (1992) Fisher’s omnibus test. In: Statistical analysis of behavioural data, an approach based on time-structured models, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 329–240Google Scholar
  21. Hamilton WD (1963) Evolution of Altruistic Behavior. Am Nat 97:354–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hammerstein P (2002) Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A protestant appeal. In: Hammerstein P (ed) Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. Dahlem University Press, Berlin, pp83–93Google Scholar
  23. Krause J, Ruxton GD, Rubenstein D (1998) Is there always an influence of shoal size on predator hunting success? J Fish Biol 52:494–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kropotkin P (1908) Mutual aid, 3rd edn. William Heinemann, LondonGoogle Scholar
  25. Kulling D, Milinski M (1992) Size-dependent predation risk and partner quality in predator inspection of sticklebacks. Anim Behav 44:949–955CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Long AG (2003) Defense pacts and international trade. J Peace Res 40:537–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Magurran AE, Seghers BH (1994a) Predator inspection behaviour Covaries with schooling tendency amongst wild guppy, Poecilia reticulata, populations in Trinidad. Behaviour 128:121–134Google Scholar
  28. Magurran AE, Seghers BH (1994b) Sexual conflict as a consequence of ecology — evidence from guppy, Poecilia reticulata, populations in Trinidad. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 255:31–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Magurran AE, Seghers BH, Shaw PW, Carvalho GR (1995) The behavioural diversity and evolution of guppy, Poecilia reticulata, populations in Trinidad. Adv Study Behav 24:155–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Milinski M (1987) Tit-for-tat in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation. Nature 325:433–435PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Milinski M, Külling D, Kettler R (1990a) Tit for tat: sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) “trusting” a cooperating partner. Behav Ecol 1:7–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Milinski M, Pfluger D, Külling D, Kettler R (1990b) Do sticklebacks cooperate repeatedly in reciprocal pairs? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 27:17–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Milinski M, Luthi JH, Eggler R, Parker GA (1997) Cooperation under predation risk: experiments on costs and benefits. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B-Biol Sci 264:831–837CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Newman MEJ (2003) The structure and function of complex networks. SIAM Rev 45:167–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pitcher TJ, Parrish JK (1993) Functions of shoaling behaviour in teleosts. In: Pitcher TJ (ed) Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edn. Chapman & Hall, London, pp363–439Google Scholar
  36. Pitcher TJ, Magurran AE, Allan JR (1983) Shifts of behaviour with shoal size in Cyprinids. In: Proceedings of the 3rd British Freshwater Fish Conference, pp220–228Google Scholar
  37. Pitcher TJ, Green DA, Magurran AE (1986) Dicing with death — predator inspection behaviour in minnow shoals. J Fish Biol 28:439–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Russell ST, Kelley JL, Graves JA, Magurran AE (2004) Kin structure and shoal composition dynamics in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Oikos 106:520–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Seghers BH (1974) Schooling behaviour in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): an evolutionary response to predation. Evolution 28:486–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Trivers RL (1971) Evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Trivers R (2004) Mutual benefits at all levels of life. Science 304:964–965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ward AJW, Hart PJB (2003) The effects of kin and familiarity on interactions between fish. Fish Fish 4:348–358Google Scholar
  43. Ward AJW, Botham MS, Hoare DJ, James R, Broom M, Godin JGJ, Krause J (2002) Association patterns and shoal fidelity in the three-spined stickleback. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B-Biol Sci 269:2451–2455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wilkinson GS (1984) Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature 308:181–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wilson DS (1975) Theory of group selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 72:143–146PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wilson DS, Dugatkin LA (1997) Group selection and assortative interactions. Am Nat 149:336–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • D. P. Croft
    • 1
    Email author
  • R. James
    • 2
  • P. O. R. Thomas
    • 1
  • C. Hathaway
    • 1
  • D. Mawdsley
    • 2
  • K. N. Laland
    • 3
  • J. Krause
    • 1
  1. 1.School of BiologyUniversity of LeedsLeedsUK
  2. 2.Department of PhysicsUniversity of BathBathUK
  3. 3.School of BiologyUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK

Personalised recommendations