Advertisement

Saccade accuracy as an indicator of the competition between functional asymmetries in vision

Abstract

Hemispheric specialization refers to the fact that cerebral hemispheres are not equivalent and that cognitive processes are lateralized in the brain. Although the potential links between handedness and the left hemisphere specialization for language have been widely studied, little attention has been paid to other motor preferences, such as eye dominance, that also are lateralized in the brain. For example, saccadic accuracy is higher in the hemifield contralateral to the dominant eye compared to the ipsilateral hemifield. Saccade accuracy is, however, also known to be sensitive to other functional asymmetries, such as the lateralization of visuo-spatial attention in the right hemisphere of the brain. Using a global effect paradigm in three different saccade latency ranges, we here propose to use saccade accuracy as an indicator of visual functional asymmetries. We show that for the shortest latencies, saccade accuracy is higher in the left than in the right visual hemifield, which could be due to the lateralization of visuo-spatial attention in the right hemisphere. For the longest latencies, however, saccade accuracy is higher toward the right than the left hemifield, probably due to the lateralization of local and global processing in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. These results could have a major impact on studies designed to measure the degree of lateralization of individuals. We here discuss both the theoretical and clinical contributions of these results.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Access options

Buy single article

Instant unlimited access to the full article PDF.

US$ 39.95

Price includes VAT for USA

Subscribe to journal

Immediate online access to all issues from 2019. Subscription will auto renew annually.

US$ 199

This is the net price. Taxes to be calculated in checkout.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. 1.

    Bayes Factors comprised between 1 and 3 bring anecdotal evidence for H1, values between 3 and 10 bring moderate evidence for H1, values between 10 and 30 bring strong evidence for H1, values between 30 and 100 bring very strong evidence for H1, and values above 100 bring extreme evidence for H1. In the same manner, Bayes Factors comprised between 0.33 and 1 bring anecdotal evidence for H0, values between 0.1 and 0.33 bring moderate evidence for H0, values between 0.033 and 0.1 bring strong evidence for H0, values between 0.01 and 0.033 bring very strong evidence for H0, and values below 0.01 bring extreme evidence for H0 (criteria proposed by Jeffreys, 1961, and modified by Lee and Wagenmakers, 2014).

References

  1. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J Mem Lang 68(3):255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001

  2. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67(1):1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01

  3. Becker W (1989) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Metrics. In: Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (eds) Reviews of oculomotor research, 3. Elsevier, Michigan, pp 13–66

  4. Bompas A, Sterling T, Rafal RD, Sumner P (2008) Naso-temporal asymmetry for signals invisible to the retinotectal pathway. J Neurophysiol 100(1):412–421. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90312.2008

  5. Bowers D, Heilman KM (1980) Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18(4–5):491–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(80)90151-7

  6. Brederoo SG, Nieuwenstein MR, Lorist MM, Cornelissen FW (2017) Hemispheric specialization for global and local processing: A direct comparison of linguistic and non-linguistic stimuli. Brain Cogn 119:10–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2017.09.005

  7. Carling K (2000) Resistant outlier rules and the non-Gaussian case. Comput Stat Data Anal 33(3):249–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-9473(99)00057-2

  8. Casteau S, Vitu F (2012) On the effect of remote and proximal distractors on saccadic behavior: a challenge to neural-field models. J Vision 12(12):14–14

  9. Chaumillon R, Blouin J, Guillaume A (2014) Eye dominance influences triggering action: the Poffenberger paradigm revisited. Cortex 58:86–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.009

  10. Chaumillon R, Alahyane N, Senot P, Vergne J, Lemoine-Lardennois C, Blouin J, Vergilino-Perez D (2017) Asymmetry in visual information processing depends on the strength of eye dominance. Neuropsychologia 96:129–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.015

  11. Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM (1988) Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. J Physiol 404(1):157–182

  12. Corballis PM, Funnell MG, Gazzaniga MS (1999) A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients. NeuroReport 10(10):2183

  13. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(3):215–229. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755

  14. Coren S, Hoenig P (1972) Effect of non-target stimuli upon length of voluntary saccades. Percept Mot Skills 34(2):499–508

  15. Corneil BD, Munoz DP (2014) Overt responses during covert orienting. Neuron 82(6):1230–1243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.040

  16. Deubel H, Wolf W, Hauske G (1984) The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In: Gale AG, Johnson F (eds) Advances in psychology: theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research, vol 22. Elsevier, Nottingham, pp 55–62

  17. Deubel H, Findlay JM, Jacobs AM, Brogan D (1988) Saccadic eye movements to targets defined by structure differences. In: Luer G, Lass U, Shallo-Hoffman J (eds) Eye movement research: physiological and psychological aspects. Hogrefe, Toronto, pp 107–145

  18. Durand AC, Gould GM (1910) A method of determining ocular dominance. J Am Med Assoc 55(5):369–370. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1910.04330050007004

  19. Erdogan AR, Özdikici M, Aydin MD, Aktas Ö, Dane S (2002) Right and left visual cortex areas in healthy subjects with right-and left-eye dominance. Int J Neurosci 112(5):517–523

  20. Findlay JM (1982) Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements. Vision Res 22(8):1033–1045. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90040-2

  21. Findlay JM, Brogan D, Wenban-Smith MG (1993) The spatial signal for saccadic eye movements emphasizes visual boundaries. Percept Psychophys 53(6):633–641

  22. Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (1996) Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature 382(6592):626–628. https://doi.org/10.1038/382626a0

  23. Hartigan JA, Hartigan PM (1985) The dip test of unimodality. Ann Stat 13(1):70–84

  24. Heinze HJ, Hinrichs H, Scholz M, Burchert W, Mangun GR (1998) Neural Mechanisms of Global and Local Processing: A Combined PET and ERP Study. J Cognit Neurosci 10(4):485–498. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562898

  25. Jeffreys H (1961) Theory of probability, 3rd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

  26. Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38(1):93–110

  27. Jóhannesson ÓI, Tagu J, Kristjánsson Á (2018) Asymmetries of the visual system and their influence on visual performance and oculomotor dynamics. Eur J Neurosci 48(11):3426–3445. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14225

  28. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (2000) Principles of neural science, 4th edn. Appleton & Lange, New York

  29. Kerkhoff G (2001) Spatial hemineglect in humans. Prog Neurobiol 63(1):1–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00028-9

  30. Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ (2014) Bayesian cognitive modeling: a practical course. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK

  31. Leigh RJ, Zee DS (2015) The neurology of eye movements. Oxford University Press, USA

  32. Martin M (1979) Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing. Neuropsychologia 17(1):33–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(79)90019-8

  33. Martinez A, Moses P, Frank L, Buxton R, Wong E, Stiles J (1997) Hemispneric asymmetries in global and local processing: evidence from fMRI. NeuroReport 8(7):1685

  34. Mesulam MM (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10(4):309–325. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.410100402

  35. Mesulam MM (1999) Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354(1387):1325–1346. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1999.0482

  36. Morey RD, Rouder JN (2018) Package “BayesFactor”. R package version 0.9.12-4.2. Available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/BayesFactor/index.html

  37. Nebes RD (1973) Perception of spatial relationships by the right and left hemispheres in commissurotomized man. Neuropsychologia 11(3):285–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(73)90039-0

  38. Nicholls ME, Roberts GR (2002) Can Free-Viewing Perceptual Asymmetries be Explained by Scanning, Pre-Motor or Attentional Biases? Cortex 38(2):113–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70645-2

  39. Nicholls MER, Beckman E, Churches O (2016) An investigation of the mechanisms underlying the effects of viewing distance and stimulus length on attentional asymmetries during line bisection. Atten Percept Psychophys. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1122-7

  40. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

  41. Saslow MG (1967) Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. JOSA 57(8):1024–1029. https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.57.001024

  42. Seyal M, Sato S, White BG, Porter RJ (1981) Visual evoked potentials and eye dominance. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 52(5):424–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(81)90025-0

  43. Shima H, Hasegawa M, Tachibana O, Nomura M, Yamashita J, Ozaki Y, Kado H (2010) Ocular dominance affects magnitude of dipole moment: an MEG study. NeuroReport 21(12):817–821. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833ce5d7

  44. Sparks DL (2002) The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(12):952–964. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn986

  45. Szczepanski SM, Kastner S (2013) Shifting attentional priorities: control of spatial attention through hemispheric competition. J Neurosci 33(12):5411–5421. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.2013

  46. Tagu J, Doré-Mazars K, Lemoine-Lardennois C, Vergilino-Perez D (2016) How eye dominance strength modulates the influence of a distractor on saccade accuracy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 57(2):534–543. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-18428

  47. Tagu J, Doré-Mazars K, Vergne J, Lemoine-Lardennois C, Vergilino-Perez D (2018a) Quantifying eye dominance strength—new insights into the neurophysiological bases of saccadic asymmetries. Neuropsychologia 117:530–540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.020

  48. Tagu J, Doré-Mazars K, Vergne J, Lemoine-Lardennois C, Vergilino-Perez D (2018b) Recentering bias for temporal saccades only: evidence from binocular recordings of eye movements. J Vision 18(1):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1167/18.1.10

  49. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DGM, Catani M (2011) A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat Neurosci 14(10):1245–1246. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2905

  50. Thomas NA, Nicholls MER (2018) Asymmetries in distractibility: left distractors improve reaction time performance. Scientific Rep 8(1):5157. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-23498-w

  51. Toba M-N, Cavanagh P, Bartolomeo P (2011) Attention biases the perceived midpoint of horizontal lines. Neuropsychologia 49(2):238–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.022

  52. Vallar G (1993) The anatomical basis of spatial hemineglect in humans. In: Marshall J, Robertson I (eds) Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies. Taylor and Francis, London, pp 27–60

  53. Vallar G (1998) Spatial hemineglect in humans. Trends Cognit Sci 2(3):87–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01145-0

  54. Van der Stigchel S, de Vries JP (2015) There is no attentional global effect: attentional shifts are independent of the saccade endpoint. J Vision 15:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.15.17

  55. Van der Stigchel S, Nijboer TC (2011) The global effect: what determines where the eyes land? J Eye Movement Res 4(2):1–13. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.4.2.3

  56. Van Kleeck MH (1989) Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia 27(9):1165–1178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(89)90099-7

  57. Vergilino-Perez D, Fayel A, Lemoine C, Senot P, Vergne J, Doré-Mazars K (2012) Are there any left-right asymmetries in saccade parameters? examination of latency, gain, and peak velocity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 53(7):3340. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.11-9273

  58. Vitu F (2008) About the global effect and the critical role of retinal eccentricity: Implications for eye movements in reading. J Eye Movement Res 2(3):1–18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.2.3.6

  59. White BJ, Munoz DP (2011) The superior colliculus. In: Liversedge SP, Gilchrist ID, Everling S (eds) The Oxford handbook of eye movements. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 195–213

  60. Wollenberg L, Deubel H, Szinte M (2018) Visual attention is not deployed at the endpoint of averaging saccades. Plos Biol 16(6):1–23. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2006548

  61. Zago L, Petit L, Mellet E, Jobard G, Crivello F, Joliot M, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (2016) The association between hemispheric specialization for language production and for spatial attention depends on left-hand preference strength. Neuropsychologia 93:394–406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.018

  62. Zago L, Petit L, Jobard G, Hay J, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Mellet E (2017) Pseudoneglect in line bisection judgement is associated with a modulation of right hemispheric spatial attention dominance in right-handers. Neuropsychologia 94:75–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.024

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors warmly thank Dr. Agnès Charvillat for her language help. They also gratefully acknowledge the two anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Correspondence to Jérôme Tagu.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Communicated by Carlo Alberto Marzi.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (PDF 270 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tagu, J., Doré-Mazars, K. & Vergilino-Perez, D. Saccade accuracy as an indicator of the competition between functional asymmetries in vision. Exp Brain Res (2020) doi:10.1007/s00221-019-05717-6

Download citation

Keywords

  • Saccadic eye movements
  • Saccadic accuracy
  • Global effect
  • Distractor
  • Hemispheric specialization
  • Asymmetries