Advertisement

Differential influence of habitual third-person vision of a body part on mental rotation of images of hands and feet

  • Louisa M. Edwards
  • Ryan S. Causby
  • Halton Stewart
  • Tasha R. StantonEmail author
Research Article

Abstract

Left/right judgement (LRJ) tasks involve determining the laterality of presented hand or feet images. Allocentric images (third-person perspective; 3PP) take longer to identify than egocentric images (first-person perspective; 1PP), supporting that implicit motor imagery (IMI)—mentally manoeuvring one’s body to match the shown posture—is used. While numerous cognitive processes are involved during LRJs, it remains unclear whether features of the individual (e.g., visual exposure, experience, task-dependent use) influence the type of recognition strategy used during LRJs (IMI versus non-IMI). To investigate whether an individual’s routine visual exposure to hands/feet in 3PP disrupts the typical perspective–reaction time (RT) relationship in LRJs, hand therapists, podiatrists, and healthy controls completed online LRJ tasks of hand and feet images. A group-specific reduction in RT for only allocentric images would represent a switch to non-IMI strategies. The results show that routine visual exposure to feet in 3PP (podiatrists) results in quicker RTs only for allocentric images of feet, suggesting a switch from IMI to non-IMI (e.g., visual object-based recognition) strategies. In contrast, routine visual exposure to hands in 3PP (hand therapists) does not alter RT for allocentric images, suggesting maintenance of IMI. However, hand therapists have quicker RTs (vs other groups) for egocentric hand images, supporting enhanced sensorimotor processing for the hand, consistent with task-dependent use (precise hand use). Higher accuracy in health professionals (vs control) on both tasks supports enhanced body schema. Combined, this suggests that 3PP visual exposure to body parts and task-dependent use contribute to LRJ performance/recognition strategy.

Keywords

Left/right judgement task Implicit motor imagery Visual object-based recognition Frame of reference Egocentric Allocentric 

Notes

Acknowledgements

TRS supported by a National Health & Medical Research Council Early Career Fellowship (ID1054041). We would also like to thank Dr David Butler and Mr Tim Cocks from NOIgroup Ltd for their assistance in setting up the left/right judgement task and for use of their NOIgroup research platform to host our online study.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

TRS received travel and accommodation support from Eli Lilly Ltd for speaking engagements (September 2014, unrelated to the present topic). All other authors have no conflicts to declare.

Supplementary material

221_2019_5512_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (173 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (PDF 172 KB)
221_2019_5512_MOESM2_ESM.docx (12 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (DOCX 12 KB)

References

  1. Berlucchi G, Aglioti S (1997) The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends Neurosci 20:560–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Berneiser J, Jahn G, Grothe M, Lotze M (2018) From visual to motor strategies: training in mental rotation of hands. NeuroImage 167:247–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berti A et al (2005) Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control. Science 309:488–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blanke O, Ionta S, Fornari E, Mohr C, Maeder P (2010) Mental imagery for full and upper human bodies: common right hemisphere activations and distinct extrastriate activations. Brain Topogr 23:321–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brady N, Maguinness C, Ni Choisdealbha A (2011) My hand or yours? Markedly different sensitivity to egocentric and allocentric views in the hand laterality task. PloS One 6:e23316  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023316 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cooper LA, Shepard RN (1975) Mental transformations in the identification of left and right hands. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 104:48–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Craig AD (2009) How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:59–70.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. De Simone L, Tomasino B, Marusic N, Eleopra R, Rumiati RI (2013) The effects of healthy aging on mental imagery as revealed by egocentric and allocentric mental spatial transformations. Acta Psychol 143:146–156.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. DiCarlo JJ, Cox DD (2007) Untangling invariant object recognition. Trends Cognit Sci 11:333–341.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. DiCarlo JJ, Zoccolan D, Rust NC (2012) How does the brain solve visual object recognition? Neuron 73:415–434.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Feng T, Zhang Z, Ji Z, Jia B, Li Y (2017) Selective effects of sport expertise on the stages of mental rotation tasks with object-based and egocentric transformations. Adv Cognit Psychol 13:248–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fiorio M et al (2007) Mental rotation of body parts and non-corporeal objects in patients with idiopathic cervical dystonia. Neuropsychologia 45:2346–2354.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ganesh S, van Schie HT, Cross ES, de Lange FP, Wigboldus DHJ (2015) Disentangling neural processes of egocentric and allocentric mental spatial transformations using whole-body photos of self and other. NeuroImage 116:30–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gentilucci M, Daprati E, Gangitano M (1998) Right-handers and left-handers have different representations of their own hand. Cogn Brain Res 6:185–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Habacha H, Molinaro C, Tabben M, Lejeune-Poutrain L (2014) Implementation of specific motor expertise during a mental rotation task of hands. Exp Brain Res 232:3454–3473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Habacha H, Lejeune-Poutrain L, Molinaro C (2017) Realistic stimuli reveal selective effects of motor expertise during a mental body rotation task. Am J Pscyhol 130:47–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hegarty M (2018) Ability and sex differences in spatial thinking: What does the mental rotation test really measure? Psychon Bull Rev 25:1212–1219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hodzic A, Kaas A, Muckli L, Stirn A, Singer W (2009) Distinct cortical networks for the detection and identification of human body NeuroImage 45:1264–1271  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.027
  19. Ionta S, Blanke O (2009) Differential influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet in left and right handers. Exp Brain Res 195:207–217.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-1770-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ionta S, Fourkas AD, Fiorio M, Aglioti S (2007) The influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet. Exp Brain Res 183:1–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ionta S, Perruchoud D, Draganski B, Blanke O (2012) Body context and posture affect mental imagery of hands. PloS One 7:e34382  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034382 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ionta S, Villiger M, Jutzeler CR, Freund P, Curt A, Gassert R (2016) Spinal cord injury affects the interplay between visual and sensorimotor representations of the body. Sci Rep 6:20144.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20144 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14:103–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kawamichi H, Kikuchi Y, Endo H, Takeda T, Yoshizawa S (1998) Temporal structure of implicit motor imagery in visual hand-shape discrimination as revealed by MEG. Neuro Rep 9:1127–1132Google Scholar
  25. Khooshabeh P, Hegarty M, Shipley TR (2013) Individual differences in mental rotation: piecemeal versus holistic processing. Exp Psychol 60:164–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kosslyn SM, DiGirolamo GJ, Thompson WL, Alpert NM (1998) Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 35:151–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lazar A, Lewis C, Fries P, Singer W, Nikolic D (2018) Visual exposure optimizes stimulus encoding in primary visual cortex. bioRxiv  https://doi.org/10.1101/502328 Google Scholar
  28. Mumaw RJ, Pellegrino J, Kail R, Carter P (1984) Different slopes for different folks process analysis of spatial aptitude. Memory Cognition 12:515–521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Myers A, Sowden PT (2008) Your hand or mine? The extrastriate body area NeuroImage 42:1669–1677  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.045
  30. Ni Choisdealbha A, Brady N, Maguinness C (2011) Differing roles for the dominant and non-dominant hands in the hand laterality task. Exp Brain Res 211:73–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Oldfield R (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):91–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Parsons LM (1987) Visual discrimination of abstract mirror-reflected three-dimensional objects at many orientations. Percept Psychophys 42:49–59.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211513 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Parsons LM, Fox PT (1998) The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognit Neuropsychol 15:22Google Scholar
  34. Qu F, Wang J, Zhong Y, Ye H (2018) Postural effects on the mental rotation of body-related pictures: an fMRI study. Front Psychol.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00720 Google Scholar
  35. Rizzolatti G, Fadigo I, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cogn Brain Res 3:131–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Saxe R, Jamal N, Powell L (2006) My body or yours? The effect of visual perspective on cortical body representations. Cereb Cortex 16:178–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schmid AB, Coppieters MW (2012) Left/right judgment of body parts is selectively impaired in patients with unilateral carpal tunnel syndrome. Clin J Pain 28:615–622.  https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e31823e16b9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Schmidt M, Egger F, Kieliger M, Rubeli B, Schuler J (2016) Gymnasts and orienteers display better mental rotation performance than nonathletes. J Individ Differ 37:1–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schwoebel J, Coslett HB (2005) Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body. J Cognit Neurosci 17:543–553.  https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053467587 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Shepard RN, Cooper LA (1986) Mental images and their transformations. Mental images and their transformations. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  41. Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sirigu A, Duhamel JR (2001) Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. J cognit Neurosci 13:910–919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stanton TR, Lin CW, Smeets RJ, Taylor D, Law R, Lorimer Moseley G (2012) Spatially defined disruption of motor imagery performance in people with osteoarthritis. Rheumatology 51:1455–1464.  https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kes048 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Toner J, Moran A (2015) Enhancing performance proficiency at the expert level: Considering the role of ‘somaesthetic awareness’. Psychol Sport Exerc 16:110–117.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Vatavu R, Pentiuc S, Chaillou C, Grisoni L, DeGrande S (2006) Visual recognition of hand postures for interacting with virtual environment. Paper presented at the 8th international conference on development and application systems, Suceava, RomaniaGoogle Scholar
  46. Wallwork SB, Butler DS, Fulton I, Stewart H, Darmawan I, Moseley GL (2013) Left/right neck rotation judgments are affected by age, gender, handedness and image rotation. Man Ther 18:225–230.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2012.10.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wraga M, Shepard JM, Church JA, Inati S, Kosslyn SM (2005) Imagined rotations of self versus objects: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 43:1351–1361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Xue J, Li C, Quan C, Lu Y, Yue J, Zhang C (2017) Uncovering the cognitive processes underlying mental rotation: an eye-movement study. Sci Rep 7:10076CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Health SciencesUniversity of South AustraliaAdelaideAustralia
  2. 2.Neuro Orthopaedic Institute (NOI)AdelaideAustralia
  3. 3.Neuroscience Research AustraliaRandwickAustralia

Personalised recommendations