Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 2, pp 377–388 | Cite as

Cognitive resilience after prolonged task performance: an ERP investigation

  • Endre TakácsEmail author
  • Irén Barkaszi
  • Anna Altbäcker
  • István Czigler
  • László Balázs
Research Article
  • 107 Downloads

Abstract

Deleterious consequences of cognitive fatigue might be avoided if people respond with increased effort to increased demands. In this study, we hypothesized that the effects of fatigue would be more pronounced in cognitive functions reflecting compensatory effort. Given that the P3a event-related potential is sensitive to the direction and amount of attention allocated to a stimulus array, we reasoned that compensatory effort would manifest in increased P3a amplitudes. Therefore, we compared P3a before (pre-test) and after (post-test) a 2 h long cognitively demanding (fatigue group, n = 18) or undemanding task (control group, n = 18). Two auditory tasks, a three-stimulus novelty oddball and a duration discrimination two-choice response task were presented to elicit P3a. In the fatigue group, we used the multi-attribute task battery as a fatigue-inducing task. This task draws on a broad array of attentional functions and imposed considerable workload. The control group watched mood-neutral documentary films. The fatigue manipulation was effective as subjective fatigue increased significantly in the fatigue group compared to controls. Contrary to expectations, however, fatigue failed to affect P3a in the post-test phase. Similar null effects were obtained for other neurobehavioral measures (P3b and behavioral performance). Results indicate that a moderate increase in subjective fatigue does not hinder cognitive functions profoundly. The lack of objective performance loss in the present study suggests that the cognitive system can be resilient against challenges instigated by demanding task performance.

Keywords

Mental fatigue Event-related potentials Attention Oddball Distraction Effort 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Péter Nagy for valuable contribution to data analysis and Tamás Fodor for programming the VAS-F scale.

Funding

This study was funded by a Hungarian Ministry of National Development Grant URK10297.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

221_2018_5427_MOESM1_ESM.docx (561 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 560 KB)

References

  1. Ackerman PL, Kanfer R (2009) Test length and cognitive fatigue: an empirical examination of effects on performance and test-taker reactions. J Exp Psychol Appl 15:163–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ackerman PL, Kanfer R, Shapiro SW et al (2010) Cognitive fatigue during testing: an examination of trait, time-on-task, and strategy influences. Hum Perform 23:381–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andrés P, Parmentier FBR, Escera C (2006) The effect of age on involuntary capture of attention by irrelevant sounds: a test of the frontal hypothesis of aging. Neuropsychologia 44:2564–2568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anguera JA, Bernard JA, Jaeggi SM et al (2012) The effects of working memory resource depletion and training on sensorimotor adaptation. Behav Brain Res 228:107–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barkaszi I, Czigler I, Balázs L (2013) Stimulus complexity effects on the event-related potentials to task-irrelevant stimuli. Biol Psychol 94:82–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barkaszi I, Takács E, Czigler I, Balázs L (2016) Extreme environment effects on cognitive functions: a longitudinal study in high altitude in Antarctica. Front Hum Neurosci 10:1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baumeister RF (2002) Ego depletion and self-control failure: an energy model of the self’s executive function. Self Identity 1:129–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beckers DGJ, Van Der Linden D, Smulders PGW et al (2008) Voluntary or involuntary? Control over overtime and rewards for overtime in relation to fatigue and work satisfaction. Work Stress 22:33–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Benoit CE, Solopchuk O, Borragán G, Carbonnelle A, Van Durme S, Zénon A (2018) Cognitive task avoidance correlates with fatigue-induced performance decrement but not with subjective fatigue. Neuropsychologia.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.017 Google Scholar
  10. Blain B, Hollard G, Pessiglione M (2016) Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of daylong cognitive work on economic decisions. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113:6967–6972CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boksem MAS, Meijman TF, Lorist MM (2005) Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn Brain Res 25:107–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boksem MAS, Meijman TF, Lorist MM (2006) Mental fatigue, motivation and action monitoring. Biol Psychol 72:123–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Borragán G, Slama H, Bartolomei M, Peigneux P (2017) Cognitive fatigue: a time-based resource-sharing account. Cortex 89:71–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brewer G, Spillers GJ, McMillan B, Unsworth N (2011) Extensive performance on the antisaccade task does not lead to negative transfer. Psychon Bull Rev 18:923–929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chong H, Riis JL, McGinnis SM et al (2008) To ignore or explore: top-down modulation of novelty processing. J Cogn Neurosci 20:120–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Comstock JR, Arnegard RJ (1992) The multi-attribute task battery for human operator workload and strategic behavior research. NASA Langley Research Center, Hampton, VAGoogle Scholar
  17. Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dinges DF, Powell JW (1985) Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 17:652–655CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Escera C, Corral MJ (2007) Role of mismatch negativity and novelty-P3 in involuntary auditory attention. J Psychophysiol 21:251–264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Esterman M, Reagan A, Liu G et al (2014) Reward reveals dissociable aspects of sustained attention. J Exp Psychol Gen 143:2287–2295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Friedman D, Kazmerski VA, Cycowicz YM (1998) Effects of aging on the novelty P3 during attend and ignore oddball tasks. Psychophysiology 35:508–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Friedman D, Cycowicz YM, Gaeta H (2001) The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain’s evaluation of novelty. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:355–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gergelyfi M, Jacob B, Olivier E, Zénon A (2015) Dissociation between mental fatigue and motivational state during prolonged mental activity. Front Behav Neurosci 9:176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harris WC, Hancock PA, Erik J et al (1995) Performance, workload, and fatigue changes associated with automation. Int J Aviat Psychol 5:169–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hart SG, Staveland LE (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv Psychol 52:139–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hockey GRJ (2011) A motivational control theory of cognitive fatigue. In: Ackerman PL (ed) Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications. American Psychological Association, pp 167–187.  https://doi.org/10.1037/12343-008
  27. Hopstaken JF, van der Linden D, Bakker AB, Kompier MAJ (2015a) A multifaceted investigation of the link between mental fatigue and task disengagement. Psychophysiology 52:305–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hopstaken JF, van der Linden D, Bakker AB, Kompier MAJ (2015b) The window of my eyes: task disengagement and mental fatigue covary with pupil dynamics. Biol Psychol 110:100–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kanfer R (2011) Determinants and consequences of subjective cognitive fatigue. In: Ackerman Phillip L (ed) Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications. American Psychological Association, pp 189–207.  https://doi.org/10.1037/12343-009
  30. Kato Y, Endo H, Kizuka T (2009) Mental fatigue and impaired response processes: event-related brain potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Int J Psychophysiol 72:204–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kelly SP, O’Connell RG (2013) Internal and external influences on the rate of sensory evidence accumulation in the human brain. J Neurosci 33:19434–19441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klaassen EB, Evers EAT, de Groot RHM et al (2014) Working memory in middle-aged males: age-related brain activation changes and cognitive fatigue effects. Biol Psychol 96:134–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kok A (2001) On the utility of P300 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38:557–577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lee K, Hicks G, Nino-Murcia G (1991) Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res 36:291–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Legrain V, Bruyer R, Guérit JM, Plaghki L (2005) Involuntary orientation of attention to unattended deviant nociceptive stimuli is modulated by concomitant visual task difficulty. Evidence from laser evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol 116:2165–2174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lim J, Dinges DF (2010) A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull 136:375–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lorist MM, Klein M, Nieuwenhuis S et al (2000) Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. Psychophysiology 37:614–625CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Luck SJ, Gaspelin N (2017) How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn’t). Psychophysiology 54:146–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Massar SAAS, Wester AEA, Volkerts EER, Kenemans JL (2010) Manipulation specific effects of mental fatigue: evidence from novelty processing and simulated driving. Psychophysiology 47:1119–1126Google Scholar
  40. Meijman TF (2000) The theory of the STOP-emotion: on the functionality of fatigue. Ergon Glob Qual Saf Prod 45–50Google Scholar
  41. Muller-Gass A, Macdonald M, Schröger E et al (2007) Evidence for the auditory P3a reflecting an automatic process: elicitation during highly-focused continuous visual attention. Brain Res 1170:71–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Oatley K, V TMS, Jenkins JM (1992) Human emotions: function and dysfunction. Annu Rev Psychol 43:55–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Persson J, Welsh KM, Jonides J, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2007) Cognitive fatigue of executive processes: interaction between interference resolution tasks. Neuropsychologia 45:1571–1579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Persson J, Larsson A, Reuter-Lorenz P (2013) Imaging fatigue of interference control reveals the neural basis of executive resource depletion. J Cogn Neurosci 25:338–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rozand V, Lebon F, Papaxanthis C, Lepers R (2015) Effect of mental fatigue on speed-accuracy trade-off. Neuroscience 297:219–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. SanMiguel I, Corral M-J, Escera C (2008) When loading working memory reduces distraction: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence from an auditory-visual distraction paradigm. J Cogn Neurosci 20:1131–1145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sarter M, Gehring WJ, Kozak R (2006) More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res Rev 51:145–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schomaker J, Meeter M (2015) Short- and long-lasting consequences of novelty, deviance and surprise on brain and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 55:268–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schröger E, Wolff C (1998) Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. Cogn Brain Res 7:71–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Schröger E, Giard MH, Wolff C (2000) Auditory distraction: event-related potential and behavioral indices. Clin Neurophysiol 111:1450–1460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Squires KC, Hillyard SA, Lindsay PH (1973) Vertex potentials evoked during auditory signal detection: relation to decision criteria. Percept Psychophys 14:265–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sussman ES, Winkler I, Schröger E (2003) Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality. Psychon Bull Rev 10:630–637CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tucker P, Folkard S, Macdonald I (2003) Rest breaks and accident risk. Lancet 361:680CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G et al (2010) Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep 33:47–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. van der Linden D (2011) The urge to stop: the cognitive and biological nature of acute mental fatigue. Cogn Fatigue Multidiscip Perspect Curr Res Futur Appl 149–164Google Scholar
  56. Van Der Hulst M, Geurts S (2001) Associations between overtime and psychological health in high and low reward jobs. Work Stress 15:227–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. van der Linden D, Frese M, Meijman TF (2003) Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychol (Amst) 113:45–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. van der Linden D, Massar SAA, Schellekens AFA et al (2006) Disrupted sensorimotor gating due to mental fatigue: preliminary evidence. Int J Psychophysiol 62:168–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Venables L, Fairclough SH (2009) The influence of performance feedback on goal-setting and mental effort regulation. Motiv Emot 33:63–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Verleger R, Jaśkowski P, Wascher E (2005) Evidence for an integrative role of P3b in linking reaction to perception. J Psychophysiol 19:165–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wilson GF, Caldwell JA, Russell CA (2007) Performance and psychophysiological measures of fatigue effects on aviation related tasks of varying difficulty. Int J Aviat Psychol 17:219–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Winkler I, Haufe S, Tangermann M (2011) Automatic classification of artifactual ICA-components for artifact removal in EEG signals. Behav Brain Funct 7:30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Zhang P, Chen X, Yuan P et al (2006) The effect of visuospatial attentional load on the processing of irrelevant acoustic distractors. Neuroimage 33:715–724CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural SciencesHungarian Academy of SciencesBudapestHungary
  2. 2.Institute of PsychologyEötvös Loránd UniversityBudapestHungary
  3. 3.Doctoral School of PsychologyEötvös Loránd UniversityBudapestHungary

Personalised recommendations