Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 237, Issue 1, pp 211–222 | Cite as

Cognitive load and emotional processing in psoriasis: a thermal imaging study

  • Maria Serena PanasitiEmail author
  • Giorgia Ponsi
  • Bianca Monachesi
  • Luigi Lorenzini
  • Vincenzo Panasiti
  • Salvatore Maria Aglioti
Research Article
  • 124 Downloads

Abstract

Psoriasis is a chronic dermatologic disease which is frequently associated with psychological distress. Although studies suggest a relationship between this condition and difficulties in emotion regulation, behavioral and physiological evidence about this link is scarce. We measured implicit emotion regulation abilities of psoriasis patients and a healthy control group by examining the impact of distracting emotional (positive, negative or neutral) images on a working memory task (“Emotional N-Back”) which could present high (2-back) or low (1-back) cognitive workload. Moreover, we used Functional Infrared Thermal Imaging to record participants’ facial temperature and obtain a measure of the activation of the autonomic system. Rising of temperature over the peri-orbital areas and the nose tip are believed to reflect the activation and the de-activation of the sympathetic system, respectively. Patients scored higher than controls on the “Lack of emotional clarity” sub-scale of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Compared to controls, who performed much better in the low vs. high cognitive load condition, patients showed a smaller accuracy difference between the two conditions. Moreover, patients showed less sympathetic (lower peri-orbital and higher nasal tip temperature) activity (especially in the negative and neutral blocks) during the high vs. low cognitive load condition, suggesting that the former condition might be less emotionally demanding for them. Patients benefit more than controls from the load-dependent interference effect when dealing with emotional information; thus, therapeutic techniques aiming at teaching how to use cognitive strategies to downregulate emotions might be particularly appropriated for them.

Keywords

Psoriasis Emotion regulation Thermal imaging Working memory Cognitive load 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are very thankful to the participants who took part to the study.

Funding

This study was funded by the PRIN Grant (Progetti di Ricerca di Rilevante Interesse Nazionale, Edit. 2015, Prot. 20159CZFJK), by the ERC Advanced Grant eHONESTY (Prot. 789058) awarded to SMA and by a Sapienza University Grant (Avvio alla Ricerca 2016, Prot. AR216154C9905620) awarded to MSP.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

221_2018_5416_MOESM1_ESM.docx (832 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 832 KB)

References

  1. Almeida V, Taveira S, Teixeira M et al (2017) Emotion regulation in patients with psoriasis: correlates of disability, clinical dimensions, and psychopathology symptoms. Int J Behav Med 24:563–570.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-016-9617-0 Google Scholar
  2. Asano H, Onogaki H, Muto T (2010) Stress Presumption of the Long Driving Using the Facial Thermal Image. J Robot Mech 751–757Google Scholar
  3. Balderston NL, Vytal KE, O’Connell K et al (2017) Anxiety patients show reduced working memory related dlPFC activation during safety and threat. Depress Anxiety 34:25–36.  https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22518 Google Scholar
  4. Barberis N, Cernaro V, Costa S et al (2017) The relationship between coping, emotion regulation, and quality of life of patients on dialysis. Int J Psychiatry Med.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0091217417720893 Google Scholar
  5. Basavaraj KH, Navya MA, Rashmi R (2011) Stress and quality of life in psoriasis: an update. Int J Dermatol 50:783–792Google Scholar
  6. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B (2011) Lme4. Linear mix model using S4 classes, pp 1–15Google Scholar
  7. Bates DM, Kliegl R, Vasishth S, Baayen H (2015) Parsimonious mixed models Douglas Bates. ArXiv e-print, under Revis 1–27Google Scholar
  8. Bergersen TK (1993) A search for arteriovenous anastomoses in human skin using ultrasound Doppler. Blair Acta Physiol Scand 147:195–201Google Scholar
  9. Brunetti M, Sepede G, Mingoia G et al (2010) Elevated response of human amygdala to neutral stimuli in mild post traumatic stress disorder: neural correlates of generalized emotional response. Neuroscience 168:670–679.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.024 Google Scholar
  10. Ciuluvica C, Amerio P, Fulcheri M (2014) Emotion regulation strategies and quality of life in dermatologic patients. Proc Soc Behav Sci 127:661–665.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.03.331 Google Scholar
  11. Clarke R, Johnstone T (2013) Prefrontal inhibition of threat processing reduces working memory interference. Front Hum Neurosci 7:1–17.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00228 Google Scholar
  12. Colgecen E, Celikbilek A, Keskin DT (2016) Cognitive impairment in patients with psoriasis: a cross-sectional study using the montreal cognitive assessment. Am J Clin Dermatol 17:413–419.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40257-016-0187-3 Google Scholar
  13. De Arruda LH, De Moraes AP (2001) The Impact of Psoriasis on Quality of Life. Br J Dematol 144:33–36.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaad.2008.11.793 Google Scholar
  14. Dekker J, de Groot V (2018) Psychological adjustment to chronic disease and rehabilitation—an exploration. Disabil Rehabil 40:116–120.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2016.1247469 Google Scholar
  15. Dolcos F (2006) Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. J Neurosci 26:2072–2079.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5042-05.2006 Google Scholar
  16. Dozois DJ, Dobson KS (2001) Information processing and cognitive organization in unipolar depression: specificity and comorbidity issues. J Abnorm Psychol 110:236–246.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.236 Google Scholar
  17. Ebisch SJ, Aureli T, Bafunno D et al (2012) Mother and child in synchrony: thermal facial imprints of autonomic contagion. Biol Psychol 89:123–129.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.018 Google Scholar
  18. Eimer M, Holmes A, Mcglone FP (2003) The role of spatial attention in the processing of facial expression: An ERP study of rapid brain responses to six basic emotions. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci 3:97–110.  https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.3.2.97 Google Scholar
  19. Erk S, Kleczar A, Walter H (2007) Valence-specific regulation effects in a working memory task with emotional context. Neuroimage 37:623–632.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.006 Google Scholar
  20. Esposito G, Nakazawa J, Ogawa S et al (2015) Using infrared thermography to assess emotional responses to infants. Early Child Dev Care 185:438–447.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.932153 Google Scholar
  21. Farroni T, Menon E, Rigato S, Johnson MH (2007) The perception of facial expressions in newborns. Eur J Dev Psychol 4:2–13.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620601046832 Google Scholar
  22. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-GG, Buchner A (2007) G * Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39:175–191.  https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146 Google Scholar
  23. Finlay AY, Kelly SE (1987) Psoriasis an index of disability. Clin Exp Dermatol 12:8–11Google Scholar
  24. Fortune G, Richards HL, Corrin A et al (2003) Attentional Bias for Psoriasis-Specific and Psychosocial Threat in Patients With Psoriasis. J Behav Med 26:211–225Google Scholar
  25. Gane L, Power S, Kushki A, Chau T (2011) Thermal imaging of the periorbital regions during the presentation of an auditory startle stimulus. PLoS One.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027268 Google Scholar
  26. Genno H, Ishikawa K, Kanbara O et al (1997) Using facial skin temperature to objectively evaluate sensations. Int J Ind Ergon 19:161–171.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-8141(96)00011-X Google Scholar
  27. Gotlib IH, Krasnoperova E, Neubauer Yue D, Joormann J (2004) Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol 113:127–135.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.127 Google Scholar
  28. Gratz KL, Roemer L (2004) multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 26:41–54.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94 Google Scholar
  29. Grecucci A, Giorgetta C, Wout M, Van et al (2012) Reappraising the ultimatum: an fMRI study of emotion regulation and decision making. Cereb Cortex 23:399–410.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs028 Google Scholar
  30. Gross JJ, John OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:348–362.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 Google Scholar
  31. Gyurak A, Gross JJ, Etkin A (2011) Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: a dual-process framework. Cognit Emot 25:400–412.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160 Google Scholar
  32. Hahn a. Whitehead C, Albrecht RD M, et al (2012) Hot or not? Thermal reactions to social contact. Biol Lett 8:864–867.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2012.0338 Google Scholar
  33. Holmes A, Vuilleumier P, Eimer M (2003) The processing of emotional facial expression is gated by spatial attention: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Cogn Brain Res 16:174–184.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00268-9 Google Scholar
  34. Holmes A, Kiss M, Eimer M (2006) Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces. Neurosci Lett 394:48–52.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.002 Google Scholar
  35. Hrehorów E, Salomon J, Matusiak Ł et al (2012) Patients with psoriasis feel stigmatized. Acta Derm Venereol 92:67–72.  https://doi.org/10.2340/00015555-1193 Google Scholar
  36. Icen M, Crowson C, McEvoy M et al (2009) Trends in incidence of adult-onset psoriasis over three decades: a population-based study. J Am Acad Dermatol 2009;60(3):394–401.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaad.2008.10.062 Google Scholar
  37. Innamorati M, Quinto RM, Imperatori C et al (2016) Health-related quality of life and its association with alexithymia and difficulties in emotion regulation in patients with psoriasis. Compr Psychiatry 70:200–208.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.08.001 Google Scholar
  38. Innamorati M, Quinto RM, Lester D et al (2018) Cognitive impairment in patients with psoriasis: a matched case-control study. J Psychosom Res 105:99–105.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.12.011 Google Scholar
  39. Ioannou S, Ebisch S, Aureli T et al (2013) The autonomic signature of guilt in children: a thermal infrared imaging study. PLoS One.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079440 Google Scholar
  40. Ioannou S, Morris P, Mercer H et al (2014a) Proximity and gaze influences facial temperature: a thermal infrared imaging study. Front Psychol 5:1–12.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00845 Google Scholar
  41. Ioannou S, Gallese V, Merla A (2014b) Thermal infrared imaging in psychophysiology: potentialities and limits. Psychophysiology 51:951–963.  https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12243 Google Scholar
  42. Ioannou S, Morris P, Terry S et al (2016) Sympathy crying: Insights from infrared thermal imaging on a female sample. PLoS One 11:1–15.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162749 Google Scholar
  43. Jaeger TF (2008) Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. J Mem Lang 59:434–446.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007.Categorical Google Scholar
  44. Kalisch R, Wiech K, Herrmann K, Dolan RJ (2006) Neural correlates of self-distraction from anxiety and a process model of cognitive emotion regulation. J Cognit Neurosci 18:1266–1276.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1266 Google Scholar
  45. Kellermann TS, Sternkopf MA, Schneider F et al (2012) Modulating the processing of emotional stimuli by cognitive demand. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 7:263–273.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq104 Google Scholar
  46. Kensinger EA, Corkin S (2003) Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion 3:378–393.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.378 Google Scholar
  47. Kosonogov V, De Zorzi L, Honoré J et al (2017) Facial thermal variations: a new marker of emotional arousal. PLoS One 12:e0183592.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183592 Google Scholar
  48. Kouchaki M, Wareham J (2015) Excluded and behaving unethically. Social exclusion, physiological responses, and unethical behavior. J Appl Psychol 100:547–556.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038034 Google Scholar
  49. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2008) International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gaineville, FLGoogle Scholar
  50. Lavie N (1995) Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:451–468.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451 Google Scholar
  51. Lavie N (2005) Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn Sci 9:75–82.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004 Google Scholar
  52. Lavie N (2010) Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:143–148.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410370295 Google Scholar
  53. Lavie N, Hirst A, de Fockert JW, Viding E (2004) Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:339–354.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 Google Scholar
  54. LeDoux JE (2009) Emotion Circuits in the Brain. Focus (Madison) 7:274–274.  https://doi.org/10.1176/foc.7.2.foc274 Google Scholar
  55. Lee E, Kang JI, Park IH et al (2008) Is a neutral face really evaluated as being emotionally neutral? Psychiatry Res 157:77–85.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.02.005 Google Scholar
  56. Levine J, Pavlidis I, Cooper M (2001) The face of fear. Lancet 357:1757Google Scholar
  57. Li P, Wang W, Fan C et al (2017) Distraction and expressive suppression strategies in regulation of high- and low-intensity negative emotions. Sci Rep 7:1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-12983-3 Google Scholar
  58. Lischetzke T, Eid M (2017) The functionality of emotional clarity: a process-oriented approach to understanding the relation between emotional clarity and well-being. The happy mind: cognitive contributions to well-being. Springer, Cham, pp 371–388Google Scholar
  59. Lischetzke T, Eid M, Diener E (2012) perceiving one’s own and others’ feelings around the world. J Cross Cult Psychol 43:1249–1267.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111429717 Google Scholar
  60. MacNamara A, Ferri J, Hajcak G (2011) Working memory load reduces the late positive potential and this effect is attenuated with increasing anxiety. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci 11:321–331.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0036-z Google Scholar
  61. Mancini A, Betti V, Panasiti MS et al (2011) Suffering makes you egoist: acute pain increases acceptance rates and reduces fairness during a bilateral ultimatum game. PLoS One.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026008 Google Scholar
  62. Mancini A, Betti V, Panasiti MS et al (2014) Perceiving monetary loss as due to inequity reduces behavioral and cortical responses to pain. Eur J Neurosci.  https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12582 Google Scholar
  63. Marek-Józefowicz L, Jaracz M, Placek W et al (2017) Cognitive impairment in patients with severe psoriasis. Adv Dermatol Allergol 2:120–125.  https://doi.org/10.5114/ada.2017.67074 Google Scholar
  64. Markela-Lerenc J, Kaiser S, Gölz T et al (2011) Attentional bias in depressive patients and the moderating effect of concurrent anxiety. Psychopathology 44:193–200.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000319370 Google Scholar
  65. Mastrolonardo M, Picardi A, Alicino D et al (2006) Cardiovascular reactivity to experimental stress in psoriasis: a controlled investigation. Acta Derm Venereol 86:340–344.  https://doi.org/10.2340/00015555-0099 Google Scholar
  66. Mastrolonardo M, Alicino D, Zefferino R et al (2007) Effect of psychological stress on salivary interleukin-1 b in Psoriasis. Arch Med Res 38:206–211.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arcmed.2006.09.009 Google Scholar
  67. McLean CP, Foa EB (2017) Emotions and emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Opin Psychol 14:72–77.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.10.006 Google Scholar
  68. Najmi S, Amir N, Frosio KE, Ayers C (2015) The effects of cognitive load on attention control in subclinical anxiety and generalised anxiety disorder. Cognit Emot 29:1210–1223.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.975188 Google Scholar
  69. Nakanishi R, Imai-Matsumura K (2008) Facial skin temperature decreases in infants with joyful expression. Infant Behav Dev 31:137–144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.09.001 Google Scholar
  70. Norberg J, Peira N, Wiens S (2010) Never mind the spider: late positive potentials to phobic threat at fixation are unaffected by perceptual load. Psychophysiology 47(6):1151–1158Google Scholar
  71. Nozawa A, Tacano M (2009) Correlation analysis on alpha attenuation and nasal skin temperature. J Stat Mech Theory Exp 2009:P01007.  https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-5468/2009/01/P01007 Google Scholar
  72. Or CKL, Duffy VG (2007) Development of a facial skin temperature-based methodology for non-intrusive mental workload measurement. Occup Ergon 7:83–94Google Scholar
  73. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E (2005) N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 25:46–59.  https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20131 Google Scholar
  74. Panasiti MS, Puzzo I, Chakrabarti B (2016a) Autistic traits moderate the impact of reward learning on social behaviour. Autism Res.  https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1523 Google Scholar
  75. Panasiti MS, Cardone D, Pavone EF et al (2016b) Thermal signatures of voluntary deception in ecological conditions. Sci Rep 6:35174.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep35174 Google Scholar
  76. Paolini D, Alparone FR, Cardone D et al (2016) “The face of ostracism”: the impact of the social categorization on the thermal facial responses of the target and the observer. Acta Psychol (Amst) 163:65–73.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.11.001 Google Scholar
  77. Patel N, Vytal K, Pavletic N et al (2016) Interaction of threat and verbal working memory in adolescents. Psychophysiology 53:518–526.  https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12582 Google Scholar
  78. Pavlidis I, Levine J (2002) Thermal image analysis for polygraph testing. IEEE Eng Med Biol Mag 21:56–64.  https://doi.org/10.1109/MEMB.2002.1175139 Google Scholar
  79. Pavlidis I, Levine J, Baukol P (2000) Thermal imaging for anxiety detection. Proc IEEE Work Comput Vis Beyond Vis Spectr Methods Appl Cat NoPR 00640:104–109.  https://doi.org/10.1109/CVBVS.2000.855255 Google Scholar
  80. Pavlidis I, Eberhardt NL, Levine J (2002) Seeing through the face of deception. Nature 415:35.  https://doi.org/10.1038/415035a Google Scholar
  81. Pavlidis I, Dowdall J, Sun N et al (2007) Interacting with human physiology. Comput Vis Image Underst 108:150–170.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cviu.2006.11.018 Google Scholar
  82. Peira N, Golkar A, Larsson M, Wiens S (2010) What you fear will appear: detection of schematic spiders in spider fear. Exp Psychol 57:470–475.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000058 Google Scholar
  83. Pessoa L (2009) How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn Sci 13:160–166.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006 Google Scholar
  84. Picardi A, Abeni D, Melchi CF et al (2000) Psychiatric morbidity in dermatological out-patients: an issue to be recognized. Br J Dermatol 143:983–991.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2133.2000.03831.x Google Scholar
  85. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM (2000) Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  86. Ponsi G, Panasiti MS, Scandola M, Aglioti SM (2015) Influence of warmth and competence on the promotion of safe in-group selection: SCM and social categorization of faces. Q J Exp Psychol 0218:1–44.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1084339 Google Scholar
  87. Ponsi G, Panasiti MS, Rizza G, Aglioti SM (2017) Thermal facial reactivity patterns predict social categorization bias triggered by unconscious and conscious emotional stimuli. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0908 Google Scholar
  88. Richard FD, Bond CF, Stokes-Zoota JJ (2003) One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Rev Gen Psychol 7:331–363.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331 Google Scholar
  89. Richards HL, Fortune DG, Griffiths CE, Main CJ (2001) The contribution of perceptions of stigmatisation to disability in patients with psoriasis. J Psychosom Res 50(1):11–15Google Scholar
  90. Riva P, Romero Lauro LJ, Vergallito A et al (2015) Electrified emotions: modulatory effects of transcranial direct stimulation on negative emotional reactions to social exclusion. Soc Neurosci 10:46–54.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.946621 Google Scholar
  91. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK (2005) The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Dev Neuropsychol 28:573–594.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2 Google Scholar
  92. Salazar-López E, Domínguez E, Juárez Ramos V et al (2015) The mental and subjective skin: Emotion, empathy, feelings and thermography. Conscious Cognit 34:149–162.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.003 Google Scholar
  93. Sand A, Wiens S (2011) Processing of unattended, simple negative pictures resists perceptual load. Neuroreport 22(7):348–352Google Scholar
  94. Sanders KM, Akiyama T (2018) The vicious cycle of itch and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 87:17–26.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.009 Google Scholar
  95. Schmitt J, Wozel G (2005) The psoriasis area and severity index is the adequate criterion to define severity in chronic plaque-type psoriasis. Dermatology 210:194–199.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000083509 Google Scholar
  96. Seidel UK, Gronewold J, Volsek M et al (2014) Physical, cognitive and emotional factors contributing to quality of life, functional health and participation in community dwelling in chronic kidney disease. PLoS One.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091176 Google Scholar
  97. Shastri D, Merla A, Tsiamyrtzis P, Pavlidis I (2009) Imaging facial signs of neurophysiological responses. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 56:477–484.  https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2008.2003265 Google Scholar
  98. Sighinolfi C, Norcini A, Rocco L (2010) Difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS): the Italian translation and adaptation. Psicoter Cognit Comport 16:141–170Google Scholar
  99. Somerville LH, Kim H, Johnstone T et al (2004) Human amygdala responses during presentation of happy and neutral faces: correlations with state anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 55:897–903.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.007 Google Scholar
  100. Terracciano A, Mccrae RR Jr, Costa PT Jr (2003) Factorial and construct validity of the Italian Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Eur J Psychol Assess 19:131–141.  https://doi.org/10.1027//1015-5759.19.2.131.Factorial Google Scholar
  101. Tsiamyrtzis P, Dowdall J, Shastri D et al (2005) Imaging facial physiology for the detection of deceit. Int J Comput Vis 71:197–214.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11263-006-6106-y Google Scholar
  102. Twenge JM, Ciarocco NJ, Baumeister RF et al (2007) Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:56–66.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56 Google Scholar
  103. Uher R, Brooks SJ, Bartholdy S et al (2014) Increasing cognitive load reduces interference from masked appetitive and aversive but not neutral stimuli. PLoS One.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094417 Google Scholar
  104. Van Dillen LF, Derks B (2012) Working memory load reduces facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP study. Emotion 12:1340–1349.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028624 Google Scholar
  105. Van Dillen LF, Koole SL (2009) How automatic is “automatic vigilance”? The role of working memory in attentional interference of negative information. Cogn Emot 23:1106–1117.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802338178 Google Scholar
  106. Van Dillen LF, Heslenfeld DJ, Koole SL (2009) Tuning down the emotional brain: an fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. Neuroimage 45:1212–1219.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016 Google Scholar
  107. Van Dillen LF, Papies EK, Hofmann W (2013) Turning a blind eye to temptation: how cognitive load can facilitate self-regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol 104:427–443.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031262 Google Scholar
  108. Vari C, Velotti P, Zavattini GC et al (2013) Emotion regulation strategies in patients with psoriasis. J Psychosom Res 74:560.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.03.084 Google Scholar
  109. Vuilleumier P, Huang YM (2009) Emotional attention uncovering the mechanisms of affective biases in perception. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18:148–152.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01626.x Google Scholar
  110. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2001) Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30(3):829–841Google Scholar
  111. Vytal K, Cornwell B, Arkin N, Grillon C (2012) Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: from impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. Psychophysiology 49:842–852.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01358.x Google Scholar
  112. Vytal KE, Arkin NE, Overstreet C et al (2016) Induced-anxiety differentially disrupts working memory in generalized anxiety disorder. BMC Psychiatry 16:62.  https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0748-2 Google Scholar
  113. Williams LM, Barton MJ, Kemp AH et al (2005) Distinct amygdala-autonomic arousal profiles in response to fear signals in healthy males and females. Neuroimage 28:618–626.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.035 Google Scholar
  114. Zhou Y, Tsiamyrtzis P, Lindner P et al (2013) Spatiotemporal smoothing as a basis for facial tissue tracking in thermal imaging. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 60:1280–1289.  https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2012.2232927 Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Maria Serena Panasiti
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Giorgia Ponsi
    • 1
    • 2
  • Bianca Monachesi
    • 1
    • 2
  • Luigi Lorenzini
    • 1
    • 2
  • Vincenzo Panasiti
    • 3
  • Salvatore Maria Aglioti
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologySapienza University of RomeRomeItaly
  2. 2.IRCCS Santa Lucia FoundationRomeItaly
  3. 3.Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery UnitCampus Bio-Medico University of RomeRomeItaly

Personalised recommendations