Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 12, pp 3391–3403 | Cite as

Emotional visual stimuli affect the evaluation of tactile stimuli presented on the arms but not the related electrodermal responses

  • Roberta EtziEmail author
  • Massimiliano Zampini
  • Georgiana Juravle
  • Alberto Gallace
Research Article

Abstract

Considering the wealth of recent studies on affective touch, to date, little research addressed the role of the other sensory modalities in the modulation of hedonic tactile perception. Here, we investigated the behavioral and electrodermal signature of the interaction between simultaneously presented visual and tactile stimuli. In three experiments, participants were presented with emotional pictures (international affective picture system; IAPS), while their forearm was gently stroked by means of different tactile textures (i.e., sandpaper, satin, tinfoil, abrasive sponge, and skin-to-skin contact). In Experiment 1, the participants evaluated the pleasantness of the tactile stimulation received, while in Experiment 2 they evaluated the pictures emotional valence. In Experiment 3 the participants rated the pleasantness, the smoothness and the softness of the textures; skin conductance responses (SCRs) were also measured. In sum, the results revealed that while the visual valence ratings were not modulated by the tactile stimulation, the hedonic and sensory tactile ratings were modulated by the visual presentation of both positively and negatively valenced pictures, as well as by neutral pictures. The modulatory effects occurring during visuo-tactile interactions might thus be not necessarily reciprocal. Moreover, the SCRs were not differently affected by the visuo-tactile or tactile conditions of stimulus presentation, suggesting a dissociation between behavioral and electrodermal effects in multisensory interactions.

Keywords

Hedonic touch IAPS pictures Multisensory interaction Skin conductance response CT afferents 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Clara Piliego for help with data collection.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

  1. Ackerley R, Wasling HB, Liljencrantz J, Olausson H, Johnson RD, Wessberg J (2014) Human C-tactile afferents are tuned to the temperature of a skin-stroking caress. J Neurosci 34:2879–2883PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Alves H, Koch A, Unkelbach C (2017) Why good is more alike than bad: Processing implications. Trends Cognit Sci 21:69–79Google Scholar
  3. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD (2001) Bad is stronger than good. Rev Gen Psychol 5:323–370Google Scholar
  4. Bellan V, Reverberi C, Gallace A (2012) Evidence for ‘Visual Enhancement of Touch’ mediated by visual displays and its relationship with body ownership. Haptics: perception, devices, mobility, and communication. lecture notes in computer science, 7282, 58–66Google Scholar
  5. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML (2015) Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86:646–664PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Björnsdotter M, Löken L, Olausson H, Vallbo Å, Wessberg J (2009) Somatotopic organization of gentle touch processing in the posterior insular cortex. J Neurosci 29:9314–9320PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Björnsdotter M, Morrison I, Olausson H (2010) Feeling good: on the role of C fiber mediated touch in interoception. Exp Brain Res 207:149–155PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA, Lang PJ (2008) The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45:602–607PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Craig AD (2003) Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:500–505PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Crisinel AS, Spence C (2010) As bitter as a trombone: synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:1994–2002PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Croy I, D’Angelo S, Olausson H (2014) Reduced pleasant touch appraisal in the presence of a disgusting odor. PLoS One 9:e92975PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. De Wied M, Verbaten M, N (2001) Affective picture processing, attention, and pain tolerance. Pain 90:13–172Google Scholar
  13. Dematte ML, Sanabria D, Sugarman R, Spence C (2006) Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and touch. Chem Senses 31:291–300PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Ditzen B, Neumann ID, Bodenmann G, von Dawans B, Turner RA, Ehlert U, Heinrichs M (2007) Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32:565–574PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Domínguez-Borràs J, Rieger SW, Corradi-Dell’Acqua C, Neveu R, Vuilleumier P (2016) Fear spreading across senses: visual emotional events alter cortical responses to touch, audition, and vision. Cereb Cortex 27:68–82PubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Dunbar RI (2010) The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:260–268PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Ellingsen DM, Wessberg J, Chelnokova O, Olausson H, Laeng B, Leknes S (2014) In touch with your emotions: oxytocin and touch change social impressions while others’ facial expressions can alter touch. Psychoneuroendocrinology 39:11–20PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Ellingsen DM, Leknes S, Løseth G, Wessberg J, Olausson H (2016) The neurobiology shaping affective touch: expectation, motivation, and meaning in the multisensory context. Front Psychol 6:1986PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Eriksson M, Storm H, Fremming A, Schollin J (2008) Skin conductance compared to a combined behavioural and physiological pain measure in newborn infants. Acta Paediatr 97:27–30PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH (2004) Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cognit Sci 8:162–169Google Scholar
  22. Essick G, James A, McGlone FP (1999) Psychophysical assessment of the affective components of non-painful touch. Neuroreport 10:2083–2087PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cognit Sci 15:85–93Google Scholar
  24. Etzi R, Gallace A (2016) The arousing power of everyday materials: an analysis of the physiological and behavioral responses to visually and tactually presented textures. Exp Brain Res 234:1659–1666PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Etzi R, Spence C, Gallace A (2014) Textures that we like to touch: an experimental study of aesthetic preferences for tactile stimuli. Conscious Cogn 29:178–188PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Etzi R, Spence C, Zampini M, Gallace A (2016) When sandpaper is ‘kiki’ and satin is ‘bouba’: an exploration of the associations between words, emotional states, and the tactile attributes of everyday materials. Multisens Res 29:133–155PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Etzi R, Ferrise F, Bordegoni M, Zampini M, Gallace A (2018) The effect of visual and auditory information on the perception of pleasantness and roughness of virtual surfaces. Multisens Res 31:501–522Google Scholar
  28. Feldman R (2012) Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm Behav 61:380–391PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Feldman R, Eidelman AI (2003) Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 45:274–281PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Feldman R, Singer M, Zagoory O (2010) Touch attenuates infants’ physiological reactivity to stress. Dev Sci 13:271–278PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Field T (2010) Touch for socioemotional and physical well-being: a review. Dev Rev 30:367–383Google Scholar
  32. Field T (2014) Touch. MIT pressGoogle Scholar
  33. Francis S, Rolls ET, Bowtell R, McGlone F, O’Doherty J, Browning A, Smith E (1999) The representation of pleasant touch in the brain and its relationship with taste and olfactory areas. Neuroreport 10:453–459PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Gallace A, Spence C (2010) The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:246–259PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Gallace A, Spence C (2014) In touch with the future: The sense of touch from cognitive neuroscience to virtual reality. University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  36. Gallace A, Spence C (2016) Social touch. In: Olausson H, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F (eds) affective touch and the neurophysiology of CT afferents. Springer, LondonGoogle Scholar
  37. Gallace A, Auvray M, Tan HZ, Spence C (2006) Visual transients impair the detection of tactile changes: a novel case of crossmodal change blindness? Neurosci Lett. 34:246–259Google Scholar
  38. Gazzola V, Spezio ML, Etzel JA, Castelli F, Adolphs R, Keysers C (2012) Primary somatosensory cortex discriminates affective significance in social touch. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109:E1657–E1666Google Scholar
  39. Gordon I, Voos AC, Bennett RH, Bolling DZ, Pelphrey KA, Kaiser MD (2013) Brain mechanisms for processing affective touch. Hum Brain Mapp 34:914–922PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Grewen KM, Anderson BJ, Girdler SS, Light KC (2003) Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behav Med 29:123–130PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Guest S, Essick G, Dessirier JM, Blot K, Lopetcharat K, McGlone F (2009) Sensory and affective judgments of skin during inter-and intrapersonal touch. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 130:115–126Google Scholar
  42. Guest S, Dessirier JM, Mehrabyan A, McGlone F, Essick G, Gescheider G, Fontana A, Xiong R, Ackerley R, Blot K (2011) The development and validation of sensory and emotional scales of touch perception. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:531–550PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Harlow HF (1958) The nature of love. Am Psychol 13:673–685Google Scholar
  44. Harlow HF, Zimmermann RR (1959) Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science 130:421–432PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Hartcher-O’Brien J, Gallace A, Krings B, Koppen C, Spence C (2008) When vision ‘extinguishes’ touch in neurologically-normal people: extending the Colavita visual dominance effect. Exp Brain Res 186:643–658PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Hartcher-O’Brien J, Levitan C, Spence C (2010) Extending visual dominance over touch for input off the body. Brain Res 1362:48–55PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Hecht D, Reiner M (2009) Sensory dominance in combinations of audio, visual and haptic stimuli. Exp Brain Res 193:307–314PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Hertenstein MJ, Verkamp JM, Kerestes AM, Holmes RM (2006a) The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 132:5–94PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Hertenstein MJ, Keltner D, App B, Bulleit BA, Jaskolka AR (2006b) Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion 6:528–533PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Hertenstein MJ, Holmes R, McCullough M, Keltner D (2009) The communication of emotion via touch. Emotion 9:566–573PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Kenntner-Mabiala R, Pauli P (2005) Affective modulation of brain potentials to painful and nonpainful stimuli. Psychophysiology 42:559–567PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Kirsch LP, Krahé C, Blom N, Crucianelli L, Moro V, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A (2018) Reading the mind in the touch: Neurophysiological specificity in the communication of emotions by touch. Neuropsychologia 116:136–149PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Kringelbach ML (2010) The hedonic brain: a functional neuroanatomy of human pleasure. In: Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC (eds) Pleasures of the brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 202–221Google Scholar
  54. Lang PJ, Greenwald MK, Bradley MM, Hamm AO (1993) Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology 30:261–273PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1999) International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings, 2. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, GainesvilleGoogle Scholar
  56. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2008) International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8, University of Florida, Gainesville, FLGoogle Scholar
  57. Lindgren L, Westling G, Brulin C, Lehtipalo S, Andersson M, Nyberg L (2012) Pleasant human touch is represented in pregenual anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 59:3427–3432PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Löken LS, Wessberg J, McGlone F, Olausson H (2009) Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nat Neurosci 12:547–548PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. McCabe C, Rolls ET, Bilderbeck A, McGlone F (2008) Cognitive influences on the affective representation of touch and the sight of touch in the human brain. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 3:97–108Google Scholar
  60. McDowell KA, Mandler G (1989) Constructions of emotion: discrepancy, arousal, and mood. Motivation Emotion 13:105–124Google Scholar
  61. McGlone F, Olausson H, Boyle JA, Jones-Gotman M, Dancer C, Guest S, Essick G (2012) Touching and feeling: Differences in pleasant touch processing between glabrous and hairy skin in humans. Eur J Neurosci 35:1782–1788PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. McGlone F, Wessberg J, Olausson H (2014) Discriminative and affective touch: Sensing and feeling. Neuron 82:737–755PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Meagher MW, Arnau RC, Rhudy JL (2001) Pain and emotion: Effects of affective picture modulation. Psychosom Med 63:79–90PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Morrison I (2016) ALE meta-analysis reveals dissociable networks for affective and discriminative aspects of touch. Hum Brain Mapp 37:1308–1320PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Morrison I, Löken LS, Olausson H (2010) The skin as a social organ. Exp Brain Res 204:305–314PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Morrison I, Björnsdotter M, Olausson H (2011) Vicarious responses to social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing speeds. J Neurosci 31:9554–9562PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Nardelli M, Greco A, Bianchi M, Scilingo EP, Valenza G (2016) On the pleasantness of a haptic stimulation: How different textures can be recognized through heart rate variability nonlinear analysis. In: Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 2016 IEEE 38th Annual International Conference, pp. 3560–3563.Google Scholar
  68. Nummenmaa L, Tuominen L, Dunbar R, Hirvonen J, Manninen S, Arponen E, Sams M (2016) Social touch modulates endogenous µ-opioid system activity in humans. Neuroimage 138:242–247PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Olausson HW, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, Morin C, Wallin BG, Starck G, Ekholm S, Strigo I, Worsley K, Vallbo ÅB, Bushnell MC (2002) Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 5:900–904PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Olausson HW, Cole J, Vallbo ÅB, McGlone F, Elam M, Krämer HH, Rylander K, Wessberg J, Bushnell MC (2008) Unmyelinated tactile afferents have opposite effects on insular and somatosensory cortical processing. Neurosci Lett 436:128–132PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Olausson H, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F (2016) Affective touch and the neurophysiology of CT afferents. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  72. Pawling R, Cannon PR, McGlone FP, Walker SC, Shirinyan D, Lieberman M (2017a) C-tactile afferent stimulating touch carries a positive affective value. PLoS One 12:e0173457PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  73. Pawling R, Trotter PD, McGlone FP, Walker SC (2017b) A positive touch: C-tactile afferent targeted skin stimulation carries an appetitive motivational value. Biol Psychol 129:186–194PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Perini I, Olausson H, Morrison I (2015) Seeking pleasant touch: neural correlates of behavioral preferences for skin stroking. Front Behav Neurosci 9:8PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  75. Poliakoff E, Miles E, Li X, Blanchette I (2007) The effect of visual threat on spatial attention to touch. Cognition 102:405–414PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein RM (1976) Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychol Rev 83:157–171PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Ramachandran VS, Brang D (2008) Tactile-emotion synesthesia. Neurocase 14:390–399PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Rhudy JL, McCabe KM, Williams AE (2007) Affective modulation of autonomic reactions to noxious stimulation. Int J Psychophysiol 63:105–109PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Rhudy JL, Bartley EJ, Williams AE (2010) Habituation, sensitization, and emotional valence modulation of pain responses. Pain 148:320–327PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Ribeiro RL, Teixeira-Silva F, Pompéia S, Bueno OFA (2007) IAPS includes photographs that elicit low-arousal physiological responses in healthy volunteers. Physiol Behav 91:671–675PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Shi Z, Jia L, Müller HJ (2012) Modulation of tactile duration judgments by emotional pictures. Front Integr Neurosci 6:24PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  82. Sinnett S, Spence C, Soto-Faraco S (2007) Visual dominance and attention: the Colavita effect revisited. Percep Psychophys 69:673–686Google Scholar
  83. Sliz D, Smith A, Wiebking C, Northoff G, Hayley S (2012) Neural correlates of a single-session massage treatment. Brain Imaging Behavior 6:77–87PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Spence C, Gallace A (2011) Multisensory design: Reaching out to touch the consumer. Psychol Market 28:267–308Google Scholar
  85. Suvilehto JT, Glerean E, Dunbar RI, Hari R, Nummenmaa L (2015) Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112:13811–13816Google Scholar
  86. Taylor SE (1991) Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychol Bull 110:67–85PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Triscoli C, Olausson H, Sailer U, Ignell H, Croy I (2013) CT-optimized skin stroking delivered by hand or robot is comparable. Front Behav Neurosci 7:208PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  88. Triscoli C, Ackerley R, Sailer U (2014) Touch satiety: differential effects of stroking velocity on liking and wanting touch over repetitions. PLoS One 9:e113425PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  89. Triscoli C, Croy I, Steudte-Schmiedgen S, Olausson H, Sailer U (2017) Heart rate variability is enhanced by long—lasting pleasant touch at CT-optimized velocity. Biol Psychol 128:71–81PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Vallbo ÅB, Olausson H, Wessberg J (1999) Unmyelinated afferents constitute a second system coding tactile stimuli of the human hairy skin. J Neurophysiol 81:2753–2763PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Velasco C, Woods AT, Deroy O, Spence Ch (2015) Hedonic mediation of the crossmodal correspondence between taste and shape. Food Qual Preference 41:151–158Google Scholar
  92. von Mohr M, Kirsch LP, Fotopoulou A (2017) The soothing function of touch: affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Sci Rep 7:13516Google Scholar
  93. Welch RB, Warren DH (1980) Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychol Bull 88:638–667PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. World Medical Association (2013) Declaration of Helsinki world medical association declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Med Assoc 210:2191–2194Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology and Milan Center for Neuroscience (NeuroMI)University of Milano-BicoccaMilanItaly
  2. 2.CIMeC, Center for Mind/Brain SciencesUniversity of TrentoRoveretoItaly
  3. 3.Department of Psychology and Cognitive ScienceUniversity of TrentoRoveretoItaly
  4. 4.ImpAct Team, U1028, INSERM, Lyon Neuroscience Research CenterLyonFrance

Personalised recommendations