Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 8, pp 2347–2362 | Cite as

The effect of aging and contextual information on manual asymmetry in tool use

  • Tea Lulic
  • Jacquelyn M. Maciukiewicz
  • David A. Gonzalez
  • Eric A. Roy
  • Clark R. Dickerson
Research Article

Abstract

Healthy aging affects manual asymmetries in simple motor tasks, such as unilateral reaching and aiming. The effects of aging on manual asymmetries in the performance of a complex, naturalistic task are unknown, but are relevant for investigating the praxis system. This study examined how aging influences manual asymmetry in different contexts in a tool manipulation task. Fifty healthy, right-hand-dominant young (N = 29; 21.41 ± 2.87 years), and elderly (N = 21; mean: 74.14 ± 6.64 years) participants performed a ‘slicing’ gesture in response to a verbal command in two contexts: with (tool) and without the tool (pantomime). For interjoint relationships between shoulder plane of elevation and elbow flexion, a HAND × AGE × CONTEXT interaction existed (F1,43 = 4.746, p = 0.035). In pantomime, interjoint control deviated more in the left (non-dominant) than the right (dominant) limb in the elderly adult group (Wilcoxon, p = 0.010). No such differences existed in the young adult group (Wilcoxon, p = 0.471). Furthermore, contextual information reduced interjoint deviation in young adults when the task was performed with the right (dominant) hand (Wilcoxon, p = 0.001) and in the elderly adults when the task was performed with the left (non-dominant) hand (Wilcoxon, p = 0.012). The presence of the tool did not reduce interjoint deviation for the right hand in the elderly group (Wilcoxon, p = 0.064) or the left hand in the young group (Wilcoxon, p = 0.044). Deviation within trials (i.e., intrasubject deviation) in elbow flexion was higher in the elderly relative to the young adult group (p = 0.003). Finally, resultant peak velocities were smaller (p = 0.002) and cycle duration longer (p < 0.0001) in the elderly adult group. This study provides novel evidence that aging affects manual asymmetries and sensorimotor control in a naturalistic task and warrants that aging research considers the context in which the task is performed.

Keywords

Manual asymmetry Aging Contextual information Pantomime Tool use Praxis 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Thank you to all participants in the study and K. Kuryllo for the assistance in programming.

Funding

This project was partially funded through Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant held by corresponding author, 311895-2016, and by a grant from the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Ontario to E. Roy. Equipment used in the grant was funded through combined support from the Canada Foundation for Innovation and the Ontario Research Fund. Dr. Dickerson is also funded through an NSERC Canada Research Chair in Shoulder Mechanics.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Albert MS, Moss M (1988) Geriatric neuropsychology. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Alt Murphy M, Willén C, Sunnerhagen KS (2011) Kinematic variables quantifying upper-extremity performance after stroke during reaching and drinking from a glass. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 25:71–80.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968310370748 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Arbib MA, Bonaiuto JB, Jacobs S, Frey SH (2009) Tool use and the distalization of the end-effector. Psychol Res 73:441–462.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0242-2 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Bellgrove MA, Phillips JG, Bradshaw JL, Gallucci RM (1998) Response (Re-)programming in aging: a kinematic analysis. J Gerontol Med Sci 53:222–227.  https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/53A.3.M222 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bennett KMB, Castiello U (1994) Reach to grasp: changes with age. J Gerontol 49:1–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown SH (1996) Control of simple arm movements in the elderly. Adv Psychol 114:27–52.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(96)80005-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown JW, Jaffe J (1975) Hypothesis on cerebral dominance. Neuropsychologia 13:107–110.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(75)90054-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bryden MP (1977) Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia 15:617–624.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(77)90067-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Buckles V (1993) Age-related slowing. In: Stelmach GE, Homberg V (eds) Sensorimotor impairment in the elderly. Kluwer Academic, Norwell, pp 73–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cabeza R (2001) Cognitive neuroscience of aging: contributions of functional neuroimagingGoogle Scholar
  11. Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging 17:85–100CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cabeza R (2004) Task-independent and task-specific age effects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention and episodic retrieval. Cereb Cortex 14:364–375.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhg133 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cicerale A, Ambron E, Lingnau A, Rumiati RI (2014) A kinematic analysis of age-related changes in grasping to use and grasping to move common objects. Acta Psychol (Amst) 151:134–142.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clark MA, Merians AS, Kothari A et al (1994) Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. Brain 117:1093–1106.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/117.5.1093 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Conover WJ, Iman RL (1982) Analysis of covariance using the rank transformation. Biometrics 38:715.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2530051 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Contreras-Vidal JL, Teulings HL, Stelmach GE (1998) Elderly subjects are impaired in spatial coordination in fine motor control. Acta Psychol (Amst) 100:25–35.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00023-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cooke JD, Brown SH, Cunningham D (1989) Kinematics of arm movements in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 10:159–165CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Darling WG, Cooke JD, Brown S (1989) Control of simple arm movements in elderly humans. Neurobiol Aging 10:149–157CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. De Renzi E, Pieczuro A, Vignolo LA (1968) Ideational apraxia: a quantitative study. Neuropsychologia 6:41–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. De Renzi E, Faglioni P, Sorgato P (1982) Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. Brain 105(Pt 2):301–12Google Scholar
  21. Dolcos F, Rice HJ, Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:819–825.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00068-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Francis KL, Spirduso WW (2000) Age differences in the expression of manual asymmetry. Exp Aging Res 26:169–180.  https://doi.org/10.1080/036107300243632 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Frey SH (2008) Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:1951–1957.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0008 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Geschwind N (1965a) Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. I Brain 88:237–94Google Scholar
  25. Geschwind N (1965b) Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. II Brain 88:585–644CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Goldenberg G (2003a) Pantomime of object use: a challenge to cerebral localization of cognitive function. Neuroimage 20:S101-106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Goldenberg G (2003b) Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia 41:1565–1573CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Goldenberg G, Hagmann S (1998) Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia 36:581–589.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00165-6 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gooderham SE, Bryden PJ (2014) Does your dominant hand become less dominant with time? The effects of aging and task complexity on hand selection. Dev Psychobiol 56:537–546.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21123 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Gravely M, Stamenova V, Roy E, Black S (2008) Aging, pantomime, and manual dexterity. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 89:e52.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ampr.2008.08.199 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gulde P, Hermsdörfer J (2017) Both hands at work: the effect of aging on upper-limb kinematics in a multi-step activity of daily living. Exp Brain Res 235:1337–1348.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4897-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Grice JW (1993) Effects of aging on planning and implementing arm movements. Psychol Aging 8:617–632CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Heath M, Westwood DA, Roy EA, Young RP (2002) Manual asymmetries in tool-use: implications for apraxia. Laterality 7:131–143.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500143000168 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Heilman KM (1973) Ideational apraxia—a re-definition. Brain 96:861–864CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hermsdörfer J, Hentze S, Goldenberg G (2006) Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia 44:1642–1652.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.023 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hermsdörfer J, Li Y, Randerath J et al (2012) Tool use without a tool: kinematic characteristics of pantomiming as compared to actual use and the effect of brain damage. Exp Brain Res 218:201–214.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3021-z CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Hermsdörfer J, Li Y, Randerath J et al (2013) Tool use kinematics across different modes of execution. Implications for action representation and apraxia. Cortex 49:184–199.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Kalisch T, Wilimzig C, Kleibel N et al (2006) Age-related attenuation of dominant hand superiority. PLoS One 1:e90.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000090 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Ketcham CJ, Seidler RD, Van Gemmert AWA, Sterlmach G (2002) Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size, and movement amplitude. Journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1:54–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kimura D, Archibald Y (1974) Motor functions of the left hemisphere. Brain 97(2):337–350CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Lacquaniti F, Soechting JF (1982) Coordination of arm and wrist motion during a reaching task. J Neurosci 2:399–408CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Liepmann H (1900) Das Krankheitschild der Apraxie (Motorischen/Asymbolie). Monatschrift Psychiatr Neurol 8:15–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Liepmann H (1908) Drei Aufsatze aus dem Apraxiegebiet. Karger, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  44. Pohl PS, Winstein CJ, Fisher BE (1996) The locus of age-related movement slowing: sensory processing in continuous goal-directed aiming. pp 94–102Google Scholar
  45. Poizner H, Mack L, Verfaellie M et al (1990) Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia: neural representations of learned movement. Brain 113:85–101CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Poizner H, Clark M, Merians AS et al (1995a) Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia. Brain 118:227–242CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Poizner H, Clark M, Merians AS et al (1995b) Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia. Brain 118:227–242.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/118.1.227 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Poizner H, Merians a S, Clark M, a et al (1998) Left hemispheric specialization for learned, skilled, and purposeful action. Neuropsychology 12:163–182.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.12.2.163 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Przybyla A, Haaland KY, Bagesteiro LB, Sainburg RL (2011) Motor asymmetry reduction in older adults. Neurosci Lett 489:99–104.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.074 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Rabbitt P (1982) Breakdown of control processes in old age. In: Field TM, Huston A, Quay HC, Troll L, Finley G (eds) Review of human development. Wiley, New York, pp 540–550Google Scholar
  51. Randerath J, Li Y, Goldenberg G, Hermsdörfer J (2009) Grasping tools: Effects of task and apraxia. Neuropsychologia 47:497–505.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.005 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Spijkers W et al (2011) From pantomime to actual use: How affordances can facilitate actual tool-use. Neuropsychologia 49:2410–2416.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.017 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Randerath J, Martin KR, Frey SH (2013) Are tool properties always processed automatically? The role of tool use context and task complexity. Cortex 49:1679–1693.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.016 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Raw RK, Wilkie RM, Culmer PR, Mon-Williams M (2012) Reduced motor asymmetry in older adults when manually tracing paths. Exp Brain Res 217:35–41.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2971-x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Rothi LJG, Heilman KM (1984) Acquisition and retention of gestures by apraxic patients. Brain Cogn 3:426–437CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Sainburg RL (2002) Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res 142:241–258.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-001-0913-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Sainburg RL, Kalakanis D (2000) Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. J Neurophysiol 83:2661–2675CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C (1993) Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. J Neurophysiol 70:2136–2147CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Salthouse T (1993) Attentional blocks are not responsible for age-related slowing. J Gerontol 48:263–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Salthouse TA, Somberg B (1982) Skilled performance: Effects of adult age and experience on elementary processes. J Exp Psychobiol 111:176–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Scharoun SM, Gonzalez DA, Bryden PJ, Roy EA (2016a) The influence of action execution on end-state comfort and underlying movement kinematics: An examination of right and left handed participants. Acta Psychol (Amst) 164:1–9.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.12.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Scharoun SM, Gonzalez DA, Roy EA, Bryden PJ (2016b) How the mode of action affects evidence of planning and movement kinematics in aging: End-state comfort in older adults. Dev Psychobiol 58:439–449.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21386 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Seidler RD, Alberts JL, Stelmach GE (2002) Changes in multi-joint performance with age. Mot Control 6:19–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB et al (2010) Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:721–733.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Seidler-Dobrin RD, Stelmach GE (1998) Persistence in visual feedback control by the elderly. Exp Brain Res 119:467–474CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Sivagnanasunderam M, Gonzalez DA, Bryden PJ et al (2015) Handedness throughout the lifespan: cross-sectional view on sex differences as asymmetries change. Front Psychol 6:1–9.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00556 Google Scholar
  67. Stamenova V, Roy EA, Black SE (2010) Associations and dissociations of transitive and intransitive gestures in left and right hemisphere stroke patients. Brain Cogn 72:483–490.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.01.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Teixeira LA (2008) Categories of manual asymmetry and their variation with advancing age. Cortex 44:707–716.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2006.10.002 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Wang J, Przybyla A, Wuebbenhorst K et al (2011) Aging reduces asymmetries in interlimb transfer of visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 210:283–290.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2631-1 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Welford AT, Norris AH, Shock NW (1969) Speed and accuracy of movement and their changes with age. Acta Psychol (Amst) 30:3–15.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90034-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Weller MP, Latimer-Sayer DT (1985) Increasing right hand dominance with age on a motor skill task. Psychol Med 15:867–872CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Wheaton LA, Hallett M (2007) Ideomotor apraxia: a review. J Neurol Sci 260:1–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2007.04.014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Wu G, van der Helm FCT, (DirkJan) Veeger HEJ et al (2005) ISB recommendation on definitions of joint coordinate systems of various joints for the reporting of human joint motion—Part II: shoulder, elbow, wrist and hand. J Biomech 38:981–992.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.05.042 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Tea Lulic
    • 1
  • Jacquelyn M. Maciukiewicz
    • 1
  • David A. Gonzalez
    • 1
  • Eric A. Roy
    • 1
  • Clark R. Dickerson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of KinesiologyUniversity of WaterlooWaterlooCanada

Personalised recommendations