Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 8, pp 2255–2262 | Cite as

You are measuring the decision to be fast, not inattention: the Sustained Attention to Response Task does not measure sustained attention

  • Jasmine S. Dang
  • Ivonne J. Figueroa
  • William S. Helton
Research Article

Abstract

The Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) has been widely used in psychological literature as a measure of vigilance (the ability to sustain attention over a prolonged period of time). This task uses a Go/No-Go paradigm and requires the participants to repetitively respond to the stimuli as quickly and as accurately as possible. Previous literature indicates that performance in SART is subjected to a “speed–accuracy trade-off” (SATO) resulting from strategy choices and from the failures of controlling motor reflexes. In this study, 36 participants (n = 36) performed a series of four SARTs. The results support the perspective of strategy choice in SART and suggest that within-subjects SATO in SART should also be acknowledged in attempting to explain SART performance. The implications of the speed–accuracy trade-off should be fully understood when the SART is being used as a measure or tool.

Keywords

Sustained Attention to Response Task Speed–accuracy trade-off Within-subject speed–accuracy trade-off (SATO) 

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors listed above declare that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alexander RA (1990) A note on averaging correlations. Bull Psychon Soc 28:335–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beckmann N, Wood RE (2017) Dynamic personality science. Integrating between-person stability and within person change. Front Psychol 8:1486CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Bedi A (2015) The effects of response probability on commission errors in high go low no-go dual response versions of the sustained attention to response task (SART). (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of CanterburyGoogle Scholar
  4. Berto R (2005) Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J Environ Psychol 25:249–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cheyne JA, Solman GJ, Carriere JS, Smilek D (2009) Anatomy of an error: a bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors. Cognition 111:98–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:8719–8724CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen J, Cohen P (1983) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Erlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  8. Dang JS, Figueroa IJ, Helton WS (2017) Determining practice effects on a cognitive flexibility assessment. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet 61:1829–1833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Finkbeiner KM, Wilson KM, Russell PN, Helton WS (2014) The effects of warning cues and attention-capturing stimuli on the sustained attention to response task. Exp Brain Res 233:1061–1068CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Head J, Helton WS (2013) Perceptual decoupling or motor decoupling? Conscious Cogn 22:913–919CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Head J, Helton WS (2016) The troubling science of neurophenomenology. Exp Brain Res 1–5Google Scholar
  12. Head J, Wilson K, Helton WS, Kemp S (2013) The role of calmness in a high-Go target detection task. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet 57:838–842 (Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Helton WS (2009) Impulsive responding and the sustained attention to response task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 31:39–47CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Helton WS, Head J, Russell PN (2011) Reliable- and unreliable-warning cues in the sustained attention to response task. Exp Brain Res 209:401–407CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Helton WS, Funke GJ, Knott BA (2014) Measuring workload in collaborative contexts: trait versus state perspectives. Hum Factors 56:322–332CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Laming DRJ (1968) Information Theory of Choice-Reaction Times. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  17. Manly T, Robertson IH, Galloway M, Hawkins K (1999) The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia 37:661–670CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. McAvinue IL, O’Keeffe F, McMackin D, Robertson IH (2005) Impaired sustained attention and error awareness in traumatic brain injury: implications for insight. Neuropsychol Rehabil 15:569–587CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Miller J (1991) Reaction time analysis with outlier exclusion: Bias varies with sample size. Q J Exp Psychol A 43:907–912CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Molenaar PCM (2004) A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Meas 2:201–218Google Scholar
  21. Peebles D, Bothell D (2004) Modelling performance in the sustained attention to response task. In: Proc ICCM 231 236. Carnegie Mellon University/University of Pittsburgh, PittsburghGoogle Scholar
  22. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J (1997) ‘Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35:747–758CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Schooler JW, Smallwood J, Christoff K, Handy TC, Reichle ED, Sayette MA (2011) Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends Cogn Sci 15:319–326PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Seli P (2016) The attention-lapse and motor decoupling accounts of SART performance are not mutually exclusive. Conscious Cogn 41:189–198CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Seli P, Jonker TR, Solman GJ, Cheyne JA, Smilek D (2013a) A methodological note on evaluating performance in a sustained-attention-to-response task. Behav Res Methods 45:355–363CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Seli P, Jonker TR, Cheyne JA, Smilek D (2013b) Enhancing SART validity by statistically controlling speed accuracy trade-offs. Front Psychol 4:265PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Smallwood J (2013) Penetrating the fog of the decoupled mind: the effects of visual salience in the sustained attention to response task. Can J Exp Psychol 67:32–40CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Smallwood J, Davies JB, Heim D, Finnigan F, Sudberry M, O’Connor R, Obonsawin M (2004) Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Conscious Cogn 13:657–690CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Teichner WH, Krebs MJ (1974) Laws of visual choice reaction time. Psychol Rev 81:75–98CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Wilson KM, Finkbeiner KM, de Joux NR, Russell PN, Helton WS (2016) Go-stimuli proportion influences response strategy in a sustained attention to response task. Exp Brain Res 234:2989–2998CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Wilson M, Joux NR, Finkbeiner KM, Russell PN, Retzler JR, Helton WS (2018) Prolonging the response movement inhibits the feed-forward motor program in the sustained attention to response task. Acta Psychol 183:75–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Wood CC, Jennings JR (1976) Speed-accuracy tradeoff functions in choice reaction time: experimental designs and computational procedures. Atten Percept Psychophys 19:92–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Woodworth RS, Schlosberg H (1954) Experimental psychology. Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  34. Zelenski JM, Larsen RJ (2000) The distribution of basic emotions in everyday life: a state and trait perspective from experience sampling data. J Res Pers 34:178–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jasmine S. Dang
    • 1
  • Ivonne J. Figueroa
    • 1
  • William S. Helton
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyGeorge Mason UniversityFairfaxUSA

Personalised recommendations