Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 7, pp 1997–2008 | Cite as

The sensory origin of the sense of effort is context-dependent

  • Florian Monjo
  • Jonathan Shemmell
  • Nicolas Forestier
Research Article

Abstract

The origin of the sense of effort has been debated for several decades and there is still no consensus among researchers regarding the underlying neural mechanisms. Some advocate that effort perception mainly arises from an efference copy originating within the brain while others believe that it is predominantly carried by muscle afferent signals. To move the debate forward, we here tested the hypothesis that there is not one but several senses of effort which depend on the way it is evaluated. For this purpose, we used two different psychophysical tests designed to test effort perception in elbow flexors. One was a bilateral isometric force-matching task in which subjects were asked to direct similar amounts of effort toward their two arms, while the other consisted of a unilateral voluntary isometric contraction in which subjects had to rate their perceived effort using a Borg scale. Throughout two distinct experiments, effort perception was evaluated before and following different tendon vibration protocols intended to differentially desensitize muscle spindles and Golgi tendon organs, and to affect the gain between the central effort and muscle contraction intensity. By putting all the results together, we found that spindle afferents played divergent roles across tasks. Namely, while they only acted as modulators of motor pathway excitability during the bilateral task, they clearly intervened as the predominant psychobiological signal of effort perception during the unilateral task. Therefore, the sensory origin of the sense of effort is not central or peripheral. Rather, it is context-dependent.

Keywords

Sense of effort Proprioception Internal models Muscle spindles Efference copy 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank all the subjects for their cooperation as well as Maximilien Bowen and Thibault Gregoire for technical assistance.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Abbiss CR, Peiffer JJ, Meeusen R, Skorski S (2015) Role of ratings of perceived exertion during self-paced exercise: what are we actually. Measuring? Sport Med 45:1235–1243.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-015-0344-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amann M (2011) Central and peripheral fatigue: interaction during cycling exercise in humans. Med Sci Sports Exerc 43:2039–2045.  https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31821f59ab CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Amann M, Blain GM, Proctor LT et al (2010) Group III and IV muscle afferents contribute to ventilatory and cardiovascular response to rhythmic exercise in humans. J Appl Physiol 109:966–976.  https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00462.2010 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Ansems GE, Allen TJ, Proske U (2006) Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles. J Physiol 576:445–455.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115097 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Avela J, Kyröläinen H, Komi P (1999) Altered reflex sensitivity after repeated and prolonged passive muscle stretching. J Appl Physiol 86:1283–1291.  https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00436748 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Barbosa TC, Vianna LC, Fernandes IA et al (2016) Intrathecal fentanyl abolishes the exaggerated blood pressure response to cycling in hypertensive men. J Physiol 594:715–725.  https://doi.org/10.1113/JP271335 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Bongiovanni LG, Hagbarth KE, Stjernberg L (1990) Prolonged muscle vibration reducing motor output in maximal voluntary contractions in man. J Physiol 423:15–26.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018008 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Borg GAV (1982) Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sport Exerc 14:377–381.  https://doi.org/10.1249/00005768-198205000-00012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Borg G (1998) Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Human Kinetics, ChampaignGoogle Scholar
  10. Borg E, Kaijser L (2006) A comparison between three rating scales for perceived exertion and two different work tests. Scand J Med Sci Sport 16:57–69.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2005.00448.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bove M, Nardone A, Schieppati M (2003) Effects of leg muscle tendon vibration on group Ia and group II reflex responses to stance perturbation in humans. J Physiol 550:617–630CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Brooks J, Allen TJ, Proske U (2013) The senses of force and heaviness at the human elbow joint. Exp Brain Res 226:617–629.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3476-6 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Brown MC, Engberg I, Matthews PB (1967) The relative sensitivity to vibration of muscle receptors of the cat. J Physiol 192:773–800.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4894(67)90031-8 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Burke D, Hagbarth KE, Lofstedt L, Wallin BG (1976) The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration during isometric contraction. J Physiol 261:695–711.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011581 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Carson RG, Riek S, Shahbazpour N (2002) Central and peripheral mediation of human force sensation following eccentric or concentric contractions. J Physiol 539:913–925.  https://doi.org/10.1013/jphysiol.2001.013385 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Christensen MS, Lundbye-Jensen J, Geertsen SS et al (2007) Premotor cortex modulates somatosensory cortex during voluntary movements without proprioceptive feedback. Nat Neurosci 10:417–419.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1873 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  18. Coppin C, Jack J, MacLennan C (1970) A method for the selective electrical activation of tendon organ afferent fibers from the cat soleus muscle. J Physiol 210:18–20Google Scholar
  19. de Morree HM, Klein C, Marcora SM (2012) Perception of effort reflects central motor command during movement execution. Psychophysiology 49:1242–1253.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01399.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fallon JB, Macefield VG (2007) Vibration sensitivity of human muscle spindles and golgi tendon organs. Muscle Nerve 36:21–29.  https://doi.org/10.1002/mus.20796 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Fetz EE, Jankowska E, Johannisson T, Lipski J (1979) Autogenetic inhibition of motoneurones by impulses in group Ia muscle spindle afferents. J Physiol 293:173–195CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Franklin DW, Wolpert DM (2011) Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron 72:425–442.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gandevia SC, McCloskey DI (1977) Effects of related sensory inputs on motor performances in man studied through changes in perceived heaviness. J Physiol 272:653–672.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp012065 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Gandevia SC, McCloskey DI, Potter EK (1980) Alterations in perceived heaviness during digital anaesthesia. J Physiol 306:365–375.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013402 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Gandevia S, Macefield G, Burke D, McKenzie D (1990) Voluntary activation of human axon in the muscle absence of afferent feedback. Brain 113:1563–1581CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gauthier GM, Nommay D, Vercher JL (1990) Ocular muscle proprioception and visual localization of targets in man. Brain 113:1857–1871.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/113.6.1857 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Goble DJ, Lewis CA, Brown SH (2006) Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback. Exp Brain Res 168:307–311.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Gregory JE, Proske U (1979) The responses of Golgi tendon organs to stimulation of different combinations of motor units. J Physiol 295:251–262.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012966 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Hampson DB, Gibson AS, Lambert MI, Noakes TD (2001) The influence of sensory cues on the perception of exertion during exercise and central regulation of exercise performance. Sport Med 31:935–952.  https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200131130-00004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hayward LF, Nielsen RP, Heckman CJ, Hutton RS (1986) Tendon vibration-induced inhibition of human and cat triceps surae group I reflexes: evidence of selective Ib afferent fiber activation. Exp Neurol 94:333–347.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(86)90107-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Heckman CJ, Condon SM, Hutton RS, Enoka RM (1984) Can Ib axons be selectively activated by electrical stimuli in human subjects? Exp Neurol 86:576–582.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(84)90090-6 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Konishi Y, Kubo J, Fukudome A (2009) Effects of prolonged tendon vibration stimulation on eccentric and concentric maximal torque and EMGs of the knee extensors. J Sport Sci Med 8:548–552Google Scholar
  33. Körding KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:244–247.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02169 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lafargue G, Paillard J, Lamarre Y, Sirigu A (2003) Production and perception of grip force without proprioception: is there a sense of effort in deafferented subjects? Eur J Neurosci 17:2741–2749.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02700.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Li W, Matin L (1992) Visual direction is corrected by a hybrid extraretinal eye position signal. Ann N Y Acad Sci 656:865–867.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25277.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Luu BL, Day BL, Cole JD, Fitzpatrick RC (2011) The fusimotor and reafferent origin of the sense of force and weight. J Physiol 589:3135–3147.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2011.208447 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Macefield VG, Gandevia SC, Bigland-Ritchie BR et al (1993) The firing rates of human motoneurones voluntarily activated in the absence of muscle afferent feedback. J Physiol 471:429–443CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. McCloskey DI, Ebeling P, Goodwin GM (1974) Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a “sense of effort. Exp Neurol 42:220–232.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(74)90019-3 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Miall RC, King D (2008) State estimation in the cerebellum. Cerebellum 7:572–576.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-008-0072-6 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Nielsen JB (2016) Human spinal motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci 8:81–101.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013913 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pageaux B (2016) Perception of effort in exercise science: definition, measurement and perspectives. Eur J Sport Sci 16:885–894.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2016.1188992 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Pollak KA, Swenson JD, Vanhaitsma TA et al (2014) Exogenously applied muscle metabolites synergistically evoke sensations of muscle fatigue and pain in human subjects. Exp Physiol 99:368–380.  https://doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.2013.075812 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Pope ZK, Defreitas JM, Pope ZK, Defreitas JM (2015) The effects of acute and prolonged muscle vibration on the function of the muscle spindle’s reflex arc the muscle spindle’s reflex arc. Somatosens Mot Res 32:254–261.  https://doi.org/10.3109/08990220.2015.1091770 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Proske U, Gandevia SC (2012) The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol Rev 92:1651–1697.  https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00048.2011 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Proske U, Gregory JE, Morgan DL et al (2004) Force matching errors following eccentric exercise. Hum Mov Sci 23:365–378.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2004.08.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Ribot-Ciscar E, Rossi-Durand C, Roll JP (1998) Muscle spindle activity following muscle tendon vibration in man. Neurosci Lett 258:147–150.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00732-0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Rogers DK, Bendrups AP, Lewis MMD (1985) Disturbed proprioception following a period of muscle vibration in humans. Neurosci Lett 57:147–152.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(85)90054-0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Sanes JN, Shadmehr R (1995) Sense of muscular effort and somesthetic afferent information in humans. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 73:223–233.  https://doi.org/10.1139/y95-033 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Scotland S, Adamo DE, Martin BJ (2014) Sense of effort revisited: relative contributions of sensory feedback and efferent copy. Neurosci Lett 561:208–212.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2013.12.041 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW (2010) Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:89–108.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Smith SA, Querry RG, Fadel PJ et al (2003) Partial blockade of skeletal muscle somatosensory afferents attenuates baroreflex resetting during exercise in humans. J Physiol 551:1013–1021.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044925 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. St Clair Gibson A, Lambert EV, Rauch LHG et al (2006) The role of information processing between the brain and peripheral physiological systems in pacing and perception of effort. Sport Med 36:705–722.  https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200636080-00006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tsay AJ, Giummarra MJ, Allen TJ, Proske U (2016) The sensory origins of human position sense. J Physiol 594:1037–1049.  https://doi.org/10.1113/JP271498 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Tucker R (2009) The anticipatory regulation of performance: the physiological basis for pacing strategies and the development of a perception-based model for exercise performance. Br J Sports Med 43:392–400.  https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2008.050799 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11:R729–R732.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Zénon A, Sidibé M, Olivier E et al (2015) Disrupting the supplementary motor area makes physical effort appear less effortful. J Neurosci 35:8737–8744.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-14.2015 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Inter-university Laboratory of Human Movement Science, UFR-SceM/STAPSUniversity Savoie Mont-BlancLe Bourget du LacFrance
  2. 2.Neural Control of Movement Laboratory, Medical and Exercise Science School of MedicineUniversity of WollongongWollongongAustralia

Personalised recommendations