Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 4, pp 1041–1052 | Cite as

Face stimulus eliminates antisaccade-cost: gaze following is a different kind of arrow

Research Article

Abstract

When a person suddenly looks in a certain direction, others seem to shift their attention to the same, looked-at, location. This common observation, that gaze-of-another seems to trigger reflexive shifts of attention within an observer, has been demonstrated in various studies. Yet just how reflexive it truly is, is an on-going controversy. Unlike most studies in which gaze cues were distractors in a cueing paradigm, the current study used gaze cues as triggers in a mixed pro- and anti-saccade task and a Posner-like discrimination task. In a set of two experiments, we investigated whether attention triggered by gaze-of-another differs from attention triggered by peripheral (exogenous) and arrow stimuli. In the first experiment, gaze cues resulted in slowed saccadic responses and in the elimination of the anti-saccade-cost associated with reflexive orienting. Pro-saccades triggered by peripheral cues had significantly fewer errors and shorter reaction times than anti-saccades. However, there was no significant difference between pro and anti-saccades triggered by gaze cues. Thus, counter to expectations, gaze did not produce reflexive shifts of overt attention. The second experiment showed that attention triggered by gaze cues is no different from attention triggered by biologically irrelevant arrow cues. They both eliminated the anti-saccade-cost and displayed prolonged reaction times. However, manual discrimination RTs showed no significant differences between gaze and peripheral cues. Together, these results suggest that neither gaze nor arrow cues trigger reflexive shifts of overt attention.

Keywords

Saccade Overt-attention Covert-attention Reflexive Gaze 

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Berger A, Henik A, Rafal R (2005) Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. J Exp Psychol Gen 134:207–221.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Birmingham E, Kingstone A (2009) Human social attention: A new look at past, present, and future investigations. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:118–140.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04468.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Driver J, Davis G, Ricciardelli P et al (1999) Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Vis cogn 6:509–540.  https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394920 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Engell AD, Nummenmaa L, Oosterhof NN et al (2010) Differential activation of frontoparietal attention networks by social and symbolic spatial cues. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5:432–440.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq008 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 16:143–149.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fischer B, Ramsperger E (1986) Human express saccades: effects of randomization and daily practice. Exp Brain Res 64:569–578.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00340494 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Fischer B, Weber H (1992) Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 89:415–424.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228257 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Fischer B, Weber H (1998) Effects of pre-cues on voluntary and reflexive saccade generation. I. Anti-cues for pro-saccades. Exp Brain Res 120:403–416.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210050414 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Forbes K, Klein RM (1996) The magnitude of the fixation offset effect with endogenously and exogenously controlled saccades. J Cogn Neurosci 8:344–352.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.4.344 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Friesen CK, Kingstone A (1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychon Bull Rev 5:490–495.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208827 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Friesen CK, Kingstone A (2003a) Covert and overt orienting to gaze direction cues and the effects of fixation offset. Neuroreport 14:489–493.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000058776.36017.5d CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Friesen CK, Kingstone A (2003b) Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects. Cognition 87:B1-10CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Friesen CK, Ristic J, Kingstone A (2004) Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:319–329.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.319 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Frischen A, Tipper SP (2004) Orienting attention via observed gaze shift evokes longer term inhibitory effects: implications for social interactions, attention, and memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:516–533.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.516 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Frischen A, Bayliss A, Tipper S (2007) Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychol Bull 133:694–724.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694.Gaze CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Green JJ, Gamble ML, Woldorff MG (2013) Resolving conflicting views: gaze and arrow cues do not trigger rapid reflexive shifts of attention. Vis Cogn 21:61–71.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2013.775209 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Greene DJ, Colich N, Iacoboni M et al (2011) Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage 56:354–362.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.031 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 74:1464–1480CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Gregory NJ, Hodgson TL (2012) Giving subjects the eye and showing them the finger: socio-biological cues and saccade generation in the anti-saccade task. Perception 41:131–147.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p7085 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Hallett PE, Adams BD (1980) The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task. Vis Res 20:329–339.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(80)90019-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hayward DA, Ristic J (2015) Exposing the cuing task: the case of gaze and arrow cues. Attent Percept Psychophys 77:1088–1104.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0877-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Henik A, Tzelgov J (1982) Is three greater than five: the relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Mem Cogn 10:389–395.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202431 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hermens F (2015) Fixation instruction influences gaze cueing. Vis Cogn 23:432–449.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2015.1042539 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hermens F, Walker R (2010) Gaze and arrow distractors influence saccade trajectories similarly. Q J Exp Psychol 63:2120–2140.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470211003718721 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hietanen JK, Nummenmaa L, Nyman MJ et al (2006) Automatic attention orienting by social and symbolic cues activates different neural networks: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 33:406–413.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.048 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hietanen JK, Leppänen JM, Nummenmaa L, Astikainen P (2008) Visuospatial attention shifts by gaze and arrow cues: an ERP study. Brain Res 1215:123–136.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2008.03.091 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hommel B, Pratt J, Colzato L, Godijn R (2001) Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychol Sci 12:360–365.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00367 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. Atten Perform IX 9:187–203Google Scholar
  29. Kingstone A, Klein RM (1993) What are human express saccades? Percept Psychophys 54:260–273.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211762 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kingstone A, Tipper C, Ristic J, Ngan E (2004) The eyes have it! An fMRI investigation. Brain Cogn 55:269–271.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.037 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Koval MJ, Thomas BS, Everling S (2005) Task-dependent effects of social attention on saccadic reaction times. Exp Brain Res 167:475–480.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0206-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kristjánsson Á (2007) Saccade landing point selection and the competition account of pro- and antisaccade generation: the involvement of visual attention—a review. Scand J Psychol 48:97–113.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00537.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kuhn G, Benson V (2007) The influence of eye-gaze and arrow pointing distractors cues on voluntary eye movements. Percept Psychophys 69:966–971.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193934 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kuhn G, Kingstone A (2009) Look away! Eyes and arrows engage oculomotor responses automatically. Atten Percept Psychophys 71:314–327.  https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.2.314 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kuhn G, Martinez LM (2012) Misdirection – Past, Present, and the Future. Front Hum Neurosci 5:1–7.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00172 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kuhn G, Tatler BW, Cole GG (2009) You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection. Vis Cogn 17:925–944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Langdon R, Smith P (2005) Spatial cueing by social versus nonsocial directional signals. Vis cogn 12:1497–1527.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000805 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Langton SRH, Bruce V (1999) Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Vis Cogn 6:541–567.  https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394939 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Liu C-L, Chiau H-Y, Tseng P et al (2010a) Antisaccade cost is modulated by contextual experience of location probability. J Neurophysiol 103:1438–1447.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00815.2009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Liu X, Park Y, Gu X, Fan J (2010b) Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus—response compatibility effects. Attent Percept Psychophys 72:1710–1720.  https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.6.1710 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ludwig CJH, Ranson A, Gilchrist ID (2008) Oculomotor capture by transient events: a comparison of abrupt onsets, offsets, motion, and flicker. J Vis 8:11CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Mansfield E, Farroni T, Johnson M (2003) Does gaze perception facilitate overt orienting? Vis Cogn 10:7–14.  https://doi.org/10.1080/713756671 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Marino BFM, Mirabella G, Actis-Grosso R et al (2015) Can we resist another person’s gaze? Front Behav Neurosci 9:1–12.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Marotta A, Pasini A, Menotti E et al (2017) Controlling attention to gaze and arrows in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res 251:148–154.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.094 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1345 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Nummenmaa L, Hietanen JK (2009) How attentional systems process conflicting cues. The superiority of social over symbolic orienting revisited. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35:1738–1754CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Parris BA, Kuhn G, Mizon GA et al (2009) Imaging the impossible: an fMRI study of impossible causal relationships in magic tricks. Neuroimage 45:1033–1039.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.036 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS, Vaughan J (1985) Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn Neuropsychol 2:211–228.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02643298508252866 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Quadflieg S, Mason MF, Macrae CN (2004) The owl and the pussycat: gaze cues and visuospatial orienting. Psychon Bull Rev 11:826–831.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196708 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Ricciardelli P, Bricolo E, Aglioti SM, Chelazzi L (2002) My eyes want to look where your eyes are looking: exploring the tendency to imitate another individual’ s gaze. Neuroreport 13:2259–2264.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000044227 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Ricciardelli P, Carcagno S, Vallar G, Bricolo E (2013) Is gaze following purely reflexive or goal-directed instead? Revisiting the automaticity of orienting attention by gaze cues. Exp Brain Res 224:93–106.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3291-5 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Ristic J, Kingstone A (2005) Taking control of reflexive social attention. Cognition.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.04.005 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Ristic J, Wright A, Kingstone A (2007) Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues. Psychon Bull Rev 14:964–969.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194129 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Scaife M, Bruner JS (1975) The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature 253:265–266CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Simon JR, Small AM (1969) Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. J Appl Psychol 53:433–435CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tipples J (2002) Eye gaze is not unique: automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychon Bull Rev 9:314–318CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Tipples J (2008) Orienting to counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Percept Psychophys 70:77–87.  https://doi.org/10.3758/PP.70.1.77 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Tomasello M (1995) Joint attention as social cognition. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  61. Wolohan FDA, Crawford TJ (2012) The anti-orienting phenomenon revisited: effects of gaze cues on antisaccade performance. Exp Brain Res 221:385–392.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3180-y CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Xu B, Tanaka JW (2015) The cued recognition task: dissociating the abrupt onset effect from the social and arrow cueing effect. Atten Percept Psychophys 77:97–110.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0763-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Zeligman L, Zivotofsky AZ (2017) Back to basics: The effects of block vs. interleaved trial administration on pro- and anti-saccade performance. PLoS One 12:e0172485.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172485 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBar-Ilan UniversityRamat GanIsrael
  2. 2.The Leslie and Susan Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research CenterBar-Ilan UniversityRamat GanIsrael

Personalised recommendations