Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 3, pp 679–689 | Cite as

Responsibility modulates the neural correlates of regret during the sequential risk-taking task

  • Lin Li
  • Zhiyuan Liu
  • Huanghuang Niu
  • Li Zheng
  • Xuemei Cheng
  • Peng Sun
  • Fanzhi Anita Zhou
  • Xiuyan GuoEmail author
Research Article

Abstract

Responsibility is a necessary prerequisite in the experience of regret. The present fMRI study investigated the modulation of responsibility on the neural correlates of regret during a sequential risk-taking task. Participants were asked to open a series of boxes consecutively and decided when to stop. Each box contained a reward, except for one containing a devil to zero participant’s gain in the trial. Once participants stopped, both collected gains and missed chances were revealed. We manipulated responsibility by setting two different contexts. In the Self (high responsibility) context, participants opened boxes and decided when to stop by themselves. In the Computer (low responsibility) context, a computer program opened boxes and decided when to stop for participants. Before each trial, participants were required to decide whether it would be a Self or a Computer context. Behaviorally, participants felt less regret (more relief) for gain outcome and more regret for the loss outcome in the high-responsibility context than low responsibility context. At the neural level, when experiencing a gain, high-responsibility trials were characterized by stronger activation in mPFC, pgACC, mOFC, and striatum with decreasing number of missed chances relative to low responsibility trials. When experiencing a loss, low responsibility trials were associated with stronger activation in dACC and bilateral insula than high-responsibility trials. Conversely, during a loss, high-responsibility trials showed more striatum activity than low responsibility trials. These results highlighted the sensitivity of the frontal region, striatum, and insula to changes in level of responsibility.

Keywords

fMRI Decision making Responsibility Striatum Regret 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [31271090; 71371180]; the National Education Research Project of China [CBA160186]; and the Key Program of the National Social Science Foundation of China [14AZD106].

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

221_2017_5165_MOESM1_ESM.docx (90 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 90 KB)

References

  1. Apicella P, Ljungberg T, Scarnati E, Schultz W (1991) Responses to reward in monkey dorsal and ventral striatum. Exp Brain Res 85:491–500CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2005) Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26:839–851CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bell DE (1982) Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Oper Res 30:961–981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boles TL, Messick DM (1995) A reverse outcome bias: the influence of multiple reference points on the evaluation of outcomes and decisions. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 61:262–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brassen S, Gamer M, Peters J, Gluth S, Christian Büchel (2012) Don’t look back in anger! Responsiveness to missed chances in successful and nonsuccessful aging. Science 336:1217516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Büchel C, Brassen S, Yacubian J, Kalisch R, Sommer T (2011) Ventral striatal signal changes represent missed opportunities and predict future choice. Neuroimage 57:1124–1130CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Camille N, Pironti VA, Dodds CM, Aitken MR, Robbins TW, Clark L (2010) Striatal sensitivity to personal responsibility in a regret-based decision-making task. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci 10:460–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chandrasekhar PVS, Capra CM, Moore S, Noussair C, Berns GS (2008) Neurobiological regret and rejoice functions for aversive outcomes. Neuroimage 39:1472–1484CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Chang LJ, Sanfey AG (2013) Great expectations: neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision-making. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 8:277–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Civai C, Crescentini C, Rustichini A, Rumiati RI (2012) Equality versus self-interest in the brain: differential roles of anterior insula and medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 62:102–112CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Connolly T, Zeelenberg M (2002) Regret in decision making. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11(215):212–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Connolly T, Ordóñez LD, Coughlan R (1997) Regret and responsibility in the evaluation of decision outcomes. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 70:73–85 (As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to look for a different version under “Related research” (further below) orfor a different version of it)CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Coricelli G, Aldo Rustichini (2010) Counterfactual thinking and emotions: regret and envy learning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365:241–247CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Coricelli G, Critchley HD, Joffily M, O’Doherty JP, Sirigu A, Dolan RJ (2005) Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nat Neurosci 8:1255–1262CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Güroğlu B, Van dBW, Rombouts SA, Crone EA (2010) Unfair? It depends: neural correlates of fairness in social context. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 5:414–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Guttentag R, Ferrell J (2004) Reality compared with its alternatives: age differences in judgments of regret and relief. Dev Psychol 40:764–775CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Haber SN, Knutson B (2010) The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:4–26Google Scholar
  18. Humphreys GW, Sui J (2016) Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN). Front Psychol 6:1726Google Scholar
  19. Izuma K, Saito DN, Sadato N (2008) Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 58:284–294CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Kelsey D, Schepanski A (1991) Regret and disappointment in taxpayer reporting decisions: an experimental study. J Behav Decis Making 4:33–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Liu Z, Li L, Zheng L, Hu Z, Roberts ID, Guo X, Yang G (2016) The neural basis of regret and relief during a sequential risk-taking task. Neuroscience 327:136–145CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Liu Z, Li L, Zheng L, Xu M, Zhou FA, Guo X (2017) Attentional deployment impacts neural response to regret. Sci Rep 7:41374CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Mandel DR (2003) Counterfactuals, emotion, and context. Cognit Emot 17:139–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Markman KD, Gavanski I, Sherman SJ, McMullen MN (1993) The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds. J Exp Soc Psychol 29:87–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mellers B (2000) Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychol Bull 126:910–924CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Nicolle A, Bach DR, Frith C, Dolan RJ (2011) Amygdala involvement in self-blame regret. Soc Neurosci 6:178–189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Ordóñez LD, Connolly T (2000) Regret and responsibility: a reply to Zeelenberg et al. (1998). Org Behav Hum Decis Process 81:132–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Roese NJ (1994) The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. J Personal Soc Psychol 66:805–818CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Roese NJ (1997) Counterfactual thinking. Psychol Bull 121:133–148CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Mackay C, Wilson JL, Jezzard P, Carter CS, Smith SM (2004) Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biol Psychiatry 55:594–602CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD (2007) dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27:2349–2356CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Wu H, Luo Y, Feng C (2016) Neural signatures of social conformity: a coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71:101–111CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Xiang T, Lohrenz T, Montague PR (2013) Computational substrates of norms and their violations during social exchange. J Neurosci 33:1099–1108CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Zeelenberg M, Pieters R (1999) Comparing service delivery to what might have been behavioral responses to regret and disappointment. J Serv Res 2:86–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Zeelenberg M, Van Dijk WW, Manstead AS (1998) Reconsidering the relation between regret and responsibility. Org Behav Hum Decis Process 74:254–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lin Li
    • 1
    • 4
  • Zhiyuan Liu
    • 2
  • Huanghuang Niu
    • 1
  • Li Zheng
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
  • Xuemei Cheng
    • 2
  • Peng Sun
    • 1
  • Fanzhi Anita Zhou
    • 5
  • Xiuyan Guo
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    Email author
  1. 1.School of Psychology and Cognitive ScienceEast China Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
  2. 2.Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance, Department of PhysicsEast China Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
  3. 3.Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics, Ministry of Education, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics, School of Psychology and Cognitive ScienceEast China Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
  4. 4.National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology EducationEast China Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
  5. 5.Zhejiang Yuying Education GroupHangzhouChina

Personalised recommendations