Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 234, Issue 9, pp 2465–2478 | Cite as

Action perception in infancy: the plasticity of 7-month-olds’ attention to grasping actions

  • Moritz M. Daum
  • Caroline Wronski
  • Annekatrin Harms
  • Gustaf Gredebäck
Research Article

Abstract

The present study investigates the plasticity of 7-month-old infants’ orienting of attention during their perception of grasping actions. Previous research has shown that when infants observe a grasping hand, they shift their attention in line with the grasping direction, which is indicated by a reliable priming effect in this direction. The mechanisms behind this priming effect are largely unknown, and it is unclear how malleable this priming effect is with respect to a brief exposure to novel action-target contingencies. In a spatial-cueing paradigm, we presented a series of training trials prior to a series of test trials. These training sequences significantly modulated infants’ attention. This suggests that action perception, when assessed through shifts of attention, is not solely based on the infants’ grasping experience but quickly adapts to context-specific observed regularities.

Keywords

Eye tracking Attention Violation of expectation Learning Action priming 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Caterina Böttcher and Kerstin Träger for the acquisition of the participants. We owe special thanks to the infants and parents who participated in this study. This research was supported by ERC Starting Grant 312292 and the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (2012-0120) awarded to Gustaf Gredebäck.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Baillargeon R, Spelke ES, Wasserman S (1985) Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition 20:191–208CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bakker M, Daum MM, Handl A, Gredebäck G (2015) Neural correlates of action perception at the onset of functional grasping. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10(6):769–776. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu119 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bakker M, Sommerville JA, Gredebäck G (2016) Enhanced neural processing of goal-directed actions after active training in 4-month-old infants. J Cogn Neurosci 28:472–482. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00909 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bernstein AS (1969) To what does the orienting response respond? Psychophysiology 6:338–350CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bertenthal BI, Clifton RK (1998) Perception and action. In: Kuhn D, Siegler RS (eds) Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 5th edn. Wiley, New York, pp 51–102Google Scholar
  6. Bertenthal BI, Boyer TW, Harding S (2014) When do infants begin to follow a point? Dev Psychol 50:2036–2048. doi: 10.1037/a0037152 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Biro S, Leslie AM (2007) Infants’ perception of goal-directed actions: development through cue-based bootstrapping. Dev Sci 10:379–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Boyer TW, Pan JS, Bertenthal BI (2011) Infants’ understanding of actions performed by mechanical devices. Cognition 121:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brandone AC, Wellman HM (2009) You can’t always get what you want: infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychol Sci 20:85–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brandone AC, Horwitz SR, Aslin RN, Wellman HM (2014) Infants’ goal anticipation during failed and successful reaching actions. Dev Sci 17:23–34. doi: 10.1111/desc.12095 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR et al (2001) Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 13:400–404. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.x PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cannon EN, Woodward AL (2012) Infants generate goal-based action predictions. Dev Sci 15:292–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01127.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cannon EN, Woodward AL, Gredebäck G et al (2012) Action production influences 12-month-old infants’ attention to others’ actions. Dev Sci 15:35–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01095.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Daum MM, Gredebäck G (2011a) The development of grasping comprehension in infancy: covert shifts of attention caused by referential actions. Exp Brain Res 208:297–307CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Daum MM, Gredebäck G (2011b) Spatial cueing by referential human gestures, arrows and mechanical devices. Int J Mind Brain Cogn 2:113–126Google Scholar
  16. Daum MM, Prinz W, Aschersleben G (2008) Encoding the goal of an object-directed but uncompleted reaching action in 6- and 9-month-old infants. Dev Sci 11:607–619CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Daum MM, Prinz W, Aschersleben G (2011) Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds. J Exp Child Psychol 108:810–818CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Daum MM, Attig M, Gunawan R et al (2012) Action seen through babies’ eyes: a dissociation between looking time and predictive gaze. Front Cogn 3:370Google Scholar
  19. Daum MM, Ulber J, Gredebäck G (2013) The development of pointing perception in infancy: effects of communicative verbal cues on covert shifts of attention. Dev Psychol 49:1898–1908. doi: 10.1037/a0031111 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Einöther SJL, Giesbrecht T (2013) Caffeine as an attention enhancer: reviewing existing assumptions. Psychopharmacology 225:251–274. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2917-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Elsner C, D’Ausilio A, Gredebäck G et al (2013) The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation. Neuropsychologia 51:488–492CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Estes K, Evans J, Alibali M, Saffran J (2007) Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychol Sci 18:254–260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Falck-Ytter T, Gredebäck G, von Hofsten C (2006) Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nat Neurosci 9:878–879CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (2002) Hand movements. In: Ramachandran VS (ed) Encyclopedia of the human brain. Academic Press, San Deigo, pp 399–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Friston K, Kilner J, Harrison L (2006) A free energy principle for the brain. J Physiol-Paris 100:70–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.10.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gallese V (2009) Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood. Psychol Res 73:486–498. doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0232-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Gampe A, Keitel A, Daum MM (2015) Intra-individual variability and continuity of action and perception measures in infants. Front Psychol Dev Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00327 Google Scholar
  28. Gergely G, Nadasdy Z, Csibra G, Biro S (1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56:165–193CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gredebäck G, Falck-Ytter T (2015) Eye movements during action observation. Perspect Cogn Sci 10(5):591–598. doi: 10.1177/1745691615589103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gredebäck G, Daum MM (2015) The microstructure of action perception in infancy: decomposing the temporal structure of social information processing. Child Dev Perspect 9:79–83. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gredebäck G, Melinder AMD (2010) Infants’ understanding of everyday social interactions: a dual process account. Cognition 114:197–206CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Gredebäck G, Örnkloo H, von Hofsten C (2006) The development of reactive saccade latencies. Exp Brain Res 173:159–164CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Gredebäck G, Stasiewicz D, Falck-Ytter T et al (2009) Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants. Dev Psychol 45:1190–1194. doi: 10.1037/a0015667 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Gredebäck G, Johnson SP, von Hofsten C (2010a) Eye tracking in infancy research. Dev Neuropsychol 35:1–19CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Gredebäck G, Melinder AMD, Daum MM (2010b) The neural basis and development of pointing comprehension. Soc Neurosci 5:441–450. doi: 10.1080/17470910903523327 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Henrichs I, Elsner C, Elsner B et al (2013) Goal certainty modulates infants’ goal-directed gaze shifts. Dev Psychol. doi: 10.1037/a0032664 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Hofer T, Hauf P, Aschersleben G (2005) Infants’ perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device. Infant Behav Dev 28:466–480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hood BM, Willen JD, Driver J (1998) Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychol Sci 9:131–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hunnius S, Bekkering H (2010) The early development of object knowledge: a study of infants’ visual anticipations during action observation. Dev Psychol 46:446–454. doi: 10.1037/a0016543 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hunnius S, Bekkering H (2014) What are you doing? How active and observational experience shape infants’ action understanding. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369:20130490. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0490 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Jakobsen KV, Frick JE, Simpson EA (2013) Look here! The development of attentional orienting to symbolic cues. J Cogn Dev 14:229–249. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.666772 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14:S103–S109CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Kanakogi Y, Itakura S (2011) Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nat Commun 2:341. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1342 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Krogh-Jespersen S, Liberman Z, Woodward AL (2015) Think fast! The relationship between goal prediction speed and social competence in infants. Dev Sci 18:815–823. doi: 10.1111/desc.12249 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Luo Y, Baillargeon R (2005) Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychol Sci 16:601–608CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Mareschal D, Quinn PC, French RM (2002) Asymmetric interference in 3- to 4-month-olds’ sequential category learning. Cogn Sci 26:377–389. doi: 10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00062-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Melzer A, Prinz W, Daum MM (2012) Production and observation of contralateral reaching: a close link by 12 months of age. Infant Behav Dev 35:570–579CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Mitrani L, Dimitrov G (1978) Pursuit eye movements of a disappearing moving target. Vis Res 18:537–539CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Nyström P (2008) The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Soc Neurosci 3:334–347CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Proverbio AM, Riva F, Zani A (2009) Observation of static pictures of dynamic actions enhances the activity of movement-related brain areas. PLoS ONE 4:e5389. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005389 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Rohlfing KJ, Longo MR, Bertenthal BI (2012) Dynamic pointing triggers shifts of visual attention in young infants. Dev Sci 15:426–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01139.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Schlottmann A, Ray E (2010) Goal attribution to schematic animals: do 6-month-olds perceive biological motion as animate? Dev Sci 13:1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00854.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Senju A, Johnson MH, Csibra G (2006) The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception. Soc Neurosci 1:220–234. doi: 10.1080/1747091060098979 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Sommerville JA, Woodward AL (2005) Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy. Cognition 95:1–30CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Sommerville JA, Woodward AL, Needham A (2005) Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition 96:B1–B11CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Sommerville JA, Blumenthal EJ, Venema K, Sage KD (2012) The body in action: the impact of self-produced action on infants’ action perception and understanding. In: Slaughter V, Brownell C (eds) Early development of body representations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 247–266Google Scholar
  58. Southgate V, Begus K (2013) Motor activation during the prediction of nonexecutable actions in infants. Psychol Sci 24:828–835. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459766 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Southgate V, Johnson MH, Osborne T, Csibra G (2009) Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants. Biol Lett 5:769–772CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  60. Stahl AE, Feigenson L (2015) Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science 348:91–94. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3799 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. von Hofsten C (1982) Eye-hand coordination in newborns. Dev Psychol 18:450–461CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. von Hofsten C (2007) Action in development. Dev Sci 10:54–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. von Hofsten C, Vishton P, Spelke ES et al (1998) Predictive action in infancy: tracking and reaching for moving objects. Cognition 67:255–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wallace PS, Whishaw IQ (2003) Independent digit movements and precision grip patterns in 1–5-month-old human infants: hand-babbling, including vacuous then self-directed hand and digit movements, precedes targeted reaching. Neuropsychologia 41:1912–1918. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00128-3 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Woodward AL (1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 69:1–34CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Woodward AL (2009) Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18:53–57CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  67. Wronski C, Daum MM (2014) Spatial orienting following directional cues in infancy. Dev Psychol 50:2020–2029. doi: 10.1037/a0037155 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Wu R, Gopnik A, Richardson DC, Kirkham NZ (2011) Infants learn about objects from statistics and people. Dev Psychol 47:1220–1229. doi: 10.1037/a0024023 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Moritz M. Daum
    • 1
    • 2
  • Caroline Wronski
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
  • Annekatrin Harms
    • 2
  • Gustaf Gredebäck
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  2. 2.Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain SciencesLeipzigGermany
  3. 3.Uppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden
  4. 4.University of Applied SciencesPotsdamGermany

Personalised recommendations