Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 233, Issue 7, pp 2215–2223 | Cite as

The effects of self-control on cognitive resource allocation during sustained attention: a transcranial Doppler investigation

  • Alexandra Becker
  • Arielle R. Mandell
  • June P. Tangney
  • Linda D. Chrosniak
  • Tyler H. Shaw
Research Article

Abstract

Vigilance, or sustained attention, is a required ability in many operational professions. While past research has consistently indicated that vigilance performance declines over time, referred to as the vigilance decrement, the theoretical mechanisms underlying the decrement continue to be explored. In the current study, trait self-control was examined to determine how this individual differences measure may contribute to the theoretical explanation of vigilance decrement. Transcranial Doppler sonography (TCD) was used as a measure of cerebral blood flow velocity (CBFV), as previous research has indicated that CBFV may index attentional resource allocation during vigilance (e.g., Shaw et al. in Hum Factors Ergon Soc 50:1619–1623, 2009). Participants performed a demanding 12-min computer-based vigilance task. Prior to the task, a validated self-report measure was used to determine trait-level self-control, and subjective workload was measured after the task was completed. Participants were divided, based upon survey responses, as either low- or high-trait self-control. Performance results showed a significant decrement across participants, but no significant main effect or interaction relating to the self-control measure was observed. Results relating to the TCD measure showed a significant decline in CBFV in the low self-control group, but no CBFV decrement was observed in the high self-control group. The subjective workload results revealed a nonsignificant trend of the low self-control group becoming more frustrated after the task. These results suggest that there are differences in the resource allocation strategies between low and high self-control participants. How trait self-control can add to an understanding of the theoretical underpinnings of sustained attention performance is discussed.

Keywords

Self-control Vigilance Cognitive resource theory Transcranial Doppler sonography Cerebral blood flow velocity 

Notes

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aaslid R (1986) Transcranial Doppler examination techniques. In: Aaslid R (ed) Transcranial Doppler sonography. Springer-Verlon, Vienna, pp 39–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barkley RA (1997) ADHD and the nature of self-control. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Baumeister RF, Alquist JL (2009) Is there a downside to good self-control? Self Identity 8:115–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM (2007) The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:351–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berardi AM, Parasuraman R, Haxby JV (2005) Sustained attention in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Developmental Neuropsychol 28(1):507–537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brocke B, Tasche KG, Beauducel A (1996) Biopsychological foundations of extraversion: differential effort reactivity and the differential P300-effect. Pers Indiv Differ 21:727–738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clark L, Iverson SD, Goodwin GM (2002) Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry 180:313–319PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. de Ridder DT, Lensvelt-Mulders G, Finkenauer C, Stok FM, Baumeister RF (2012) Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 16:76–99PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. DeWall NC, Baumeister RF, Mead NL, Vohs KD (2011) How leaders self-regulate their task performance: evidence that power promotes diligence, depletion, and disdain. J Pers Soc Psychol 100:47–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Duschek S, Schandry R (2003) Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research. Psychophysiology 40:436–454PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eysenck MW, Calvo MG (1992) Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cogn Emot 6:409–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Finomore VS, Matthews G, Shaw TH, Warm JS (2009) Predicting vigilance: a fresh look at an old problem. Ergonomics 52:791–808PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Green MW, Rogers PJ (1995) Impaired cognitive functioning during spontaneous dieting. Psychol Med 25:1003–1010PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hart SG, Staveland LE (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of empirical and theoretical research in human mental workload. Adv Psychol 52:139–183Google Scholar
  15. Helton WS, Russell PN (2011) Feature absence–presence and two theories of lapses of sustained attention. Psychol Res 75:384–391PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Helton WS, Dember WM, Warm JS, Matthews G (1999) Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: effects on vigilance performance. Curr Psychol 18:311–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Helton WS et al (2007) The abbreviated vigilance task and cerebral hemodynamics. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 29:545–552PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Helton WS, Shaw T, Warm JS, Matthews G, Hancock PA (2008) Effects of warned and unwarned demand transitions on vigilance performance and stress. Anxiety Stress Copin 21:173–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Helton WS, Matthews G, Warm JS (2009) Stress state mediation between environmental variables and performance: the case of noise and vigilance. Acta Psychol 130:204–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hill NM, Schneider W (2006) Brain changes in the development of expertise: neuroanatomical and neurophysiological evidence about skill-based adaptations. In: Ericsson KA, Charness N, Feltovich P, Hoffman RR (eds) Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 223–242Google Scholar
  21. Hitchcock et al (2003) Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task. Theor Issues Ergon Sci 4:89–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Humphreys MS, Revelle W (1984) Personality, motivation and performance. A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychol Rev 91:153–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall, Englewood CliffsGoogle Scholar
  24. Koelega HS (1992) Extraversion and vigilance performance: 30 years of inconsistencies. Psychol Bull 112:239–258PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2013) Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol Bull 139(4):870PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Langner R, Willmes K, Chatterjee A, Eickhoff SB, Sturm W (2010) Energetic effects of stimulus intensity on prolonged simple reaction-time performance. Psycholl Res 74:99–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Levin ED, Wilson W, Rose JE, McEvoy J (1996) Nicotine–haloperidol interactions and cognitive performance in schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacol 15:429–436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mackworth NH (1948) The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quart J Exp Psychol 1:6–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Manly T, Robertson IH, Galloway M, Hawkins K (1999) The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia 37:661–670PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Muraven M, Baumeister RF (2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol Bull 126:247–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nguyen C, Satterfield K, Bellows B, McKnight P, Shaw TH (2013) Assessing resource allocation during vigilance using transcranial Doppler: the effects of extraversion. Hum Factors Ergon Soc 57:818–822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ossowski U, Malinen S, Helton WS (2011) The effects of emotional stimuli on target detection: indirect and direct resource costs. Conscious Cog 20:1649–1658CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Parasuraman R (2009) Assaying individual differences in cognition with molecular genetics: theory and application. Theor Issues Ergon Sci 10:399–416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Parasuraman R, Davies DR (1984) Varieties of attention. Academic Press, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  35. Parasuraman R, Warm JS, Dember WN (1987) Vigilance: taxonomy and utility. In: Mark LS, Warm JS, Huston RL (eds) Ergonomics and human factors: recent research. Springer, New York, pp 11–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Parasuraman R, Warm JS, See JE (1998) Brain systems of vigilance. In: Parasuraman R (ed) The attentive brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 221–256Google Scholar
  37. Risko EF, Anderson N, Sarwal A, Engelhardt M, Kingstone A (2012) Everyday attention: variation in mind wandering and memory in lecture. Appl Cogn Psychol 26:234–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade I, Baddeley BT, Yiend J (1997) “Oops”: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologica 35:747–748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. See JE, Howe SR, Warm JS, Dember WN (1995) Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychol Bull 117(2):230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Seidel WT, Joschko M (1990) Evidence of difficulties in sustained attention in children with ADDH. J Abnorm Child Psychol 18:217–229PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shaw TH, Warm JS, Matthews G, Riley M, Weiler EM, Dember WN, Tripp L, Finomore V, Hollander TD (2009) Effects of sensory modality on vigilance performance and cerebral hemovelocity. Hum Factors Ergon Soc 50:1619–1623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shaw TH, Matthews G, Warm JS, Finomore VS, Silverman L, Costa PT Jr (2010) Individual differences in vigilance: personality, ability and states of stress. J Res Pers 44:297–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shaw T, Finomore V, Warm J, Matthews G (2012) Effects of regular or irregular event schedules on cerebral hemovelocity during a sustained attention task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 34:57–66PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Shaw TH, Funke ME, Dillard M, Funke GJ, Warm JS, Parasuraman R (2013a) Event-related cerebral hemodynamics reveal target-specific resource allocation for both “go” and “no-go” response-based vigilance tasks. Brain Cogn 82:265–273PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Shaw TH, Satterfield K, Ramirez R, Finomore V (2013b) Using cerebral hemovelocity to measure workload during a spatialized auditory vigilance task for novice and experienced observers. Ergonomics 56:1251–1263PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Smulders FTY, Meijer EH (2008) Extraversion and performance: a cognitive energetical approach. Pers Indiv Differ 44:475–486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stroobant N, Vingerhoets G (2000) Transcranial Doppler ultrasonography monitoring of cerebral hemodynamics during performance of cognitive tasks: a review. Neuropsychol Rev 10:213–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Szalma JL, Warm JS, Matthews G, Dember WN, Weiler EM, Meier A, Eggemeier FT (2004) Effects of sensory modality and task duration on performance, workload, and stress in sustained attention. Hum Factors 46:219–233PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL (2004) High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers 72:271–322PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Temple JG, Warm JS, Dember WN, Jones KS, LaGrange CM, Matthews G (2000) The effects of signal salience and caffeine on performance, workload and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Hum Factors 42:183–194PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Vohs K, Baumeister RF, Schmeichel BJ, Twenge JM, Nelson NM, Tice DM (2008) Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. J Pers Soc Psychol 94:883PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Warm JS, Dember WN (1998) Tests of a vigilance taxonomy. In: Hoffman RR, Sherrick MF, Warm JS (eds) Viewing psychology as a whole: the integrative science of William N. Dember. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 87–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Warm JS, Parasuraman R, Matthews G (2008) Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum Factors 50:433–441PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Warm JS, Matthews G, Parasuraman R (2009) Cerebral hemodynamics and vigilance performance. Mil Psychol 21(suppl 1):S75–S100CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyGeorge Mason UniversityFairfaxUSA

Personalised recommendations