Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 232, Issue 12, pp 4021–4033 | Cite as

Visuospatial viewpoint manipulation during full-body illusion modulates subjective first-person perspective

  • Christian Pfeiffer
  • Valentin Schmutz
  • Olaf Blanke
Research Article

Abstract

Self-consciousness is based on multisensory signals from the body. In full-body illusion (FBI) experiments, multisensory conflict was used to induce changes in three key aspects of bodily self-consciousness (BSC): self-identification (which body ‘I’ identify with), self-location (where ‘I’ am located), and first-person perspective (from where ‘I’ experience the world; 1PP). Here, we adapted a previous FBI protocol in which visuotactile stroking was administered by a robotic device (tactile stroking) and simultaneously rendered on the back of a virtual body (visual stroking) that participants viewed on a head-mounted display as if filmed from a posterior viewpoint of a camera. We compared the effects of two different visuospatial viewpoints on the FBI and thereby on these key aspects of BSC. During control manipulations, participants saw a no-body object instead of a virtual body (first experiment) or received asynchronous versus synchronous visuotactile stroking (second experiment). Results showed that within-subjects visuospatial viewpoint manipulations affected the subjective 1PP ratings if a virtual body was seen but had no effect for viewing a non-body object. However, visuospatial viewpoint had no effect on self-identification, but depended on the viewed object and visuotactile synchrony. Self-location depended on visuospatial viewpoint (first experiment) and visuotactile synchrony (second experiment). Our results show that the visuospatial viewpoint from which the virtual body is seen during FBIs modulates the subjective 1PP and that such viewpoint manipulations contribute to spatial aspects of BSC. We compare the present data with recent data revealing vestibular contributions to the subjective 1PP and discuss the multisensory nature of BSC and the subjective 1PP.

Keywords

Bodily self-consciousness Multisensory integration First-person perspective Full-body illusion Visuospatial viewpoint 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Andrea Serino for insightful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. This work was supported by Grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SINERGIA CRSII1-125135), the European Science Foundation (FP7 project VERE) and the Bertarelli Foundation. The funders had no role in the study preparation, execution, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aichhorn M, Perner J, Kronbichler M, Staffen W, Ladurner G (2006) Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? Neuroimage 30(3):1059–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.026 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arzy S, Thut G, Mohr C, Michel CM, Blanke O (2006) Neural basis of embodiment: distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area. J Neurosci 26(31):8074–8081. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0745-06.2006 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aspell JE, Lenggenhager B, Blanke O (2009) Keeping in touch with one’s self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness. PLoS ONE 4(8):e6488. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006488 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aspell JE, Lavanchy T, Lenggenhager B, Blanke O (2010) Seeing the body modulates audiotactile integration. Eur J Neurosci 31(10):1868–1873. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07210.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Astafiev SV, Stanley CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2004) Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions. Nat Neurosci 7(5):542–548. doi: 10.1038/nn1241 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21(1):37–46PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blanke O (2012) Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 13(8):556–571. doi: 10.1038/nrn3292 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Blanke O, Metzinger T (2009) Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn Sci 13(1):7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Blanke O, Mohr C (2005) Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 50(1):184–199. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.008 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blanke O, Ortigue S, Landis T, Seeck M (2002) Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature 419(6904):269–270. doi: 10.1038/419269a PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Blanke O, Landis T, Spinelli L, Seeck M (2004) Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain 127(Pt 2):243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bonferroni CE (1935) l calcolo delle assicurazioni su gruppi di teste. In: Studi in Onore del Professore Salvatore Ortu Carboni. Rome, pp 13–60Google Scholar
  13. Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391(6669):756. doi: 10.1038/35784 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Broughton R, Wasel N (1990) A text-stimuli presentation manager for the IBM PC with ipsatization correction for response sets and reaction times. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 22(4):421–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Brugger P, Agosti R, Regard M, Wieser HG, Landis T (1994) Heautoscopy, epilepsy, and suicide. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57(7):838–839PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cattell RB (1944) Psychological measurement: normative, ipsative and interactive. Psychol Rev 51:292–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. de Gelder B, Van den Stock J, Meeren HK, Sinke CB, Kret ME, Tamietto M (2010) Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(4):513–527. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.008 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. De Ridder D, Van Laere K, Dupont P, Menovsky T, Van de Heyning P (2007) Visualizing out-of-body experience in the brain. N Engl J Med 357(18):1829–1833. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa070010 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. De Saedeleer C, Vidal M, Lipshits M, Bengoetxea A, Cebolla AM, Berthoz A, Cheron G, McIntyre J (2013) Weightlessness alters up/down asymmetries in the perception of self-motion. Exp Brain Res 226(1):95–106. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3414-7 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Duenas J, Chapuis D, Pfeiffer C, Martuzzi R, Ionta S, Blanke O, Gassert R (2011) Neuroscience robotics to investigate multisensory integration and bodily awareness. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 8348–8352. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6092059
  21. Ehrsson HH (2007) The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science 317(5841):1048. doi: 10.1126/science.1142175 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Freton M, Lemogne C, Bergouignan L, Delaveau P, Lehericy S, Fossati P (2013) The eye of the self: precuneus volume and visual perspective during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain Struct Funct. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0546-2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Frith U, Frith CD (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358(1431):459–473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Frith C, Frith U (2005) Theory of mind. Curr Biol 15(17):R644–R646. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.041 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50(4):531–534. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gentile G, Guterstam A, Brozzoli C, Ehrsson HH (2013) Disintegration of multisensory signals from the real hand reduces default limb self-attribution: an fMRI study. J Neurosci 33(33):13350–13366. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1363-13.2013 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Guterstam A, Ehrsson HH (2012) Disowning one’s seen real body during an out-of-body illusion. Conscious Cogn 21(2):1037–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.018 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hänsel A, Lenggenhager B, von Kanel R, Curatolo M, Blanke O (2011) Seeing and identifying with a virtual body decreases pain perception. Eur J Pain 15(8):874–879. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.03.013 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Heydrich L, Blanke O (2013) Distinct illusory own-body perceptions caused by damage to posterior insula and extrastriate cortex. Brain 136:790–803. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws364 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ionta S, Heydrich L, Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Fornari E, Chapuis D, Gassert R, Blanke O (2011) Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron 70(2):363–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17(11):4302–4311PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kennett S, Taylor-Clarke M, Haggard P (2001) Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr Biol 11(15):1188–1191PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lackner JR (1992) Spatial orientation in weightless environments. Perception 21(6):803–812PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lackner JR, DiZio PA (2000) Aspects of body self-calibration. Trends Cogn Sci 4(7):279–288PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lackner JR, DiZio P (2005) Vestibular, proprioceptive, and haptic contributions to spatial orientation. Annu Rev Psychol 56:115–147. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142023 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O (2007) Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science 317(5841):1096–1099. doi: 10.1126/science.1143439 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Blanke O (2009) Spatial aspects of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 18(1):110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Loftus GR, Masson M (1994) Using confidence intervals in within-subjects designs. Psychon Bull Rev 1(4):476–490PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Longo MR, Iannetti GD, Mancini F, Driver J, Haggard P (2012) Linking pain and the body: neural correlates of visually induced analgesia. J Neurosci 32(8):2601–2607. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4031-11.2012 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Metzinger T (2003) Being no one. MIT Press, BostonGoogle Scholar
  41. Minnebusch DA, Daum I (2009) Neuropsychological mechanisms of visual face and body perception. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33(7):1133–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.008 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Minnebusch DA, Keune PM, Suchan B, Daum I (2010) Gradual inversion affects the processing of human body shapes. Neuroimage 49(3):2746–2755. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.046 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH (2008) If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS ONE 3(12):e3832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003832 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Petkova VI, Khoshnevis M, Ehrsson HH (2011) The perspective matters! Multisensory integration in ego-centric reference frames determines full-body ownership. Front Psychol 2:35. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00035 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pfeiffer C, Lopez C, Schmutz V, Duenas JA, Martuzzi R, Blanke O (2013) Multisensory origin of the subjective first-person perspective: visual, tactile, and vestibular mechanisms. PLoS ONE 8(4):e61751. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061751 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Romano D, Pfeiffer C, Maravita A, Blanke O (2014) Illusory self-identification with an avatar reduces arousal responses to painful stimuli. Behav Brain Res 261:275–281. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.049 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Salomon R, Lim M, Pfeiffer C, Gassert R, Blanke O (2013) Full body illusion is associated with widespread skin temperature reduction. Front Behav Neurosci 7:65. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00065 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Serino A, Alsmith A, Costantini M, Mandrigin A, Tajadura-Jimenez A, Lopez C (2013) Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 22(4):1239–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.013 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sforza A, Bufalari I, Haggard P, Aglioti SM (2010) My face in yours: visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. Soc Neurosci 5(2):148–162. doi: 10.1080/17470910903205503 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Slater M, Spanlang B, Sanchez-Vives MV, Blanke O (2010) First person experience of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 5(5):e10564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010564 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tiliket C, Ventre-Dominey J, Vighetto A, Grochowicki M (1996) Room tilt illusion. A central otolith dysfunction. Arch Neurol 53(12):1259–1264PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tsakiris M (2008) Looking for myself: current multisensory input alters self-face recognition. PLoS ONE 3(12):e4040. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004040 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tsakiris M, Haggard P (2005) The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31(1):80–91. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7(1):38–42PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Vogeley K, May M, Ritzl A, Falkai P, Zilles K, Fink GR (2004) Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J Cogn Neurosci 16(5):817–827. doi: 10.1162/089892904970799 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christian Pfeiffer
    • 1
    • 2
  • Valentin Schmutz
    • 1
    • 2
  • Olaf Blanke
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Center for Neuroprosthethics, School of Life SciencesEcole Polytechnique FédéraleLausanneSwitzerland
  2. 2.Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Brain Mind InstituteEcole Polytechnique FédéraleLausanneSwitzerland
  3. 3.Department of NeurologyUniversity HospitalGenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations