Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 232, Issue 10, pp 3301–3316 | Cite as

Haptic grasping configurations in early infancy reveal different developmental profiles for visual guidance of the Reach versus the Grasp

  • Jenni M. KarlEmail author
  • Ian Q. Whishaw
Research Article

Abstract

The Dual Visuomotor Channel theory posits that reaching consists of two movements mediated by separate but interacting visuomotor pathways that project from occipital to parietofrontal cortex. The Reach transports and orients the hand to the target while the Grasp opens and closes the hand for target purchase. Adults rely on foveal vision to synchronize the Reach and the Grasp so that the hand orients, opens, and largely closes by the time it gets to the target. Young infants produce discrete preReach and preGrasp movements, but it is unclear how these movements become synchronized under visual control throughout development. High-speed 3-D video recordings and linear kinematics were used to analyze reaching components, hand orientation, hand aperture, and grasping strategy in infants aged 4–24 months compared with adults who reached with and without vision. Infants aged 4–8 months resembled adults reaching without vision; in that, they delayed both Reach orientation and Grasp closure until after target contact, suggesting that they relied primarily on haptic cues to guide reaching. Infants aged 9–24 months oriented the Reach prior to target contact, but continued to delay the majority of Grasp closure until after target contact, suggesting that they relied on vision for the Reach versus haptics for the Grasp. Changes in sensorimotor control were associated with sequential Reach and Grasp configurations in early infancy versus partially synchronized Reach and Grasp configurations in later infancy. The results argue that (1) haptic inputs likely contribute to the initial development of separate Reach and Grasp pathways in parietofrontal cortex; (2) the Reach and the Grasp are adaptively uncoupled during development, likely to capitalize on different sensory inputs at different developmental stages; and (3) the developmental transition from haptic to visual control is asymmetrical with visual guidance of the Reach preceding that of the Grasp.

Keywords

Reach Grasp Prehension Development Haptic Dual Visuomotor Channels 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the infants and their parents for agreeing to participate in this study and Lauren A. Hall for help with data analysis and figure preparation. This research was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (JMK, IQW), Alberta Innovates-Health Solutions (JMK), and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (IQW).

Supplementary material

Supplementary material 1 (MOV 2529 kb)

Supplementary material 2 (MOV 623 kb)

Supplementary material 3 (MOV 1061 kb)

Supplementary material 4 (MOV 764 kb)

Supplementary material 5 (MOV 1137 kb)

Supplementary material 6 (MOV 3009 kb)

Supplementary material 7 (MOV 675 kb)

References

  1. Arbib MA (1981) Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In: Brooks VB (ed) Handbook of physiology, section 1, vol 2, part 2. American Psychological Society, Bethesda, pp 1449–1480Google Scholar
  2. Barrett TM, Needham A (2008) Developmental differences in infants’ use of an object’s shape to grasp it securely. Dev Psychobiol 50:97–106PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C (1998) Eye, head, and trunk control: the foundation for manual development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:515–520PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berthier NE, Carrico RL (2010) Visual information and object size in infant reaching. Infant Behav Dev 33:555–566PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bremner AJ, Hill EL, Pratt M, Rigato S, Spence C (2013) Bodily illusions in young children: developmental change in visual and proprioceptive contributions to perceived hand position. PLoS One 8:e51887PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Butterworth G, Verweij E, Hopkins B (1997) The development of prehension in infants: Halverson revisited. Br J Dev Psychol 15:223–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carrico RL, Berthier NE (2008) Vision and precision reaching in 15-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 31:62–70PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cavina-Pratesi C, Monaco S, Fattori P, Galleti C, McAdam TD, Quinlan DJ, Goodale MA, Culham JC (2010) Functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals the neural substrates of arm transport and grip formation in reach-to-grasp actions in humans. J Neurosci 30:10306–10323PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clifton RK, Muir DW, Ashmead DH, Clarkson MG (1993) Is visually guided reaching in early infancy a myth? Child Dev 64:1099–1110PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Corbetta D, Snapp-Childs W (2009) Seeing and touching: the role of sensory-motor experience on the development of infant reaching. Infant Behav Dev 32:44–58PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Culham JC, Valyear KF (2006) Human parietal cortex in action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:205–212PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Bruin N, Sacrey LA, Brown LA, Doan J, Whishaw IQ (2008) Visual guidance for hand advance but not hand withdrawal in a reach-to-eat task in adult humans: reaching is a composite movement. J Mot Behav 40:337–346PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Domellöf E, Hopkins B, Francis B, Rönnqvist L (2007) Effects of finger markers on the kinematics of reaching movements in young children and adults. J Appl Biomech 23:315–321PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Eshkol N, Wachman A (1958) Movement notation. Weidenfeld and Nicholson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  15. Fagard J (2000) Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behavior of 5- to 12-month-old human infants grasping objects of different sizes. Infant Behav Dev 23:317–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fiehler K, Rösler F (2010) Plasticity of multisensory dorsal stream functions: evidence from congenitally blind and sighted adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci 28:193–205PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fiehler K, Burke M, Bien S, Röder B, Rösler F (2009) The human dorsal action control system develops in the absence of vision. Cereb Cortex 19:1–12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Filimon F (2010) Human cortical control of hand movements: parietofrontal networks for reaching, grasping, and pointing. Neuroscientist 16:388–407PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Foroud A, Whishaw IQ (2012) The consummatory origins of visually guided reaching in human infants: a dynamic integration of whole-body and upper-limb movements. Behav Brain Res 231:343–355PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gharbawie OA, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH (2011) Cortical connections of functional zones in posterior parietal cortex and frontal cortex motor regions in new world monkeys. Cereb Cortex 21:1981–2002PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gori M, Del Viva M, Sandini G, Burr DC (2008) Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information. Curr Biol 18:694–698PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Greenman GW (1963) Visual behavior of newborn infants. In: Solnit AJ, Provence SA (eds) Modern perspectives in child development. Hallmark, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  24. Hadders-Algra M (2013) Typical and atypical development of reaching and postural control in infancy. Dev Med Child Neurol 55(Suppl 4):5–8PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hall LA, Karl JM, Thomas BL, Whishaw IQ (2014) Reach and Grasp reconfigurations reveal that proprioception assists reaching and hapsis assists grasping in peripheral vision. Exp Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3945-6
  26. Halverson HM (1931) An experimental study of prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema records. Genet Psychol Monogr 10:107–286Google Scholar
  27. Heck RH, Thomas SL, Tabata LN (2014) Examining individual change with repeated measures data. In: Multilevel and longitudinal modeling with IBM SPSS, 2nd edn. Routledge, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  28. Jeannerod M (1981) Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In: Long J, Badeley A (eds) Attention, performance IX. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 153–169Google Scholar
  29. Jeannerod M (1999) Visuomotor channels: their integration in goal-directed prehension. Hum Mov Sci 18:201–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Karl JM, Whishaw IQ (2013) Different evolutionary origins for the Reach and the Grasp: an explanation for dual visuomotor channels in primate parietofrontal cortex. Front Neurol 4:208PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Karl JM, Sacrey LA, Doan JB, Whishaw IQ (2012) Hand shaping using hapsis resembles visually guided hand shaping. Exp Brain Res 219:59–74PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Karl JM, Schneider LR, Whishaw IQ (2013) Nonvisual learning of intrinsic object properties in a reaching task dissociates grasp from reach. Exp Brain Res 225:465–477PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Konczak J, Dichgans J (1997) The development toward stereotypic arm kinematics during reaching in the first 3 years of life. Exp Brain Res 117:346–354PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kremenitzer JP, Vaughan HG, Kurtzberg D, Dowling K (1979) Smooth-pursuit eye movements in the newborn infant. Child Dev 50:442–448Google Scholar
  35. Lockman JJ, Ashmead DH, Bushnell EW (1984) The development of anticipatory hand orientation during infancy. J Exp Child Psychol 37:176–186PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Loenneker T, Klaver P, Bucher K, Lichtensteiger J, Imfeld A, Martin E (2010) Microstructural development: organizational differences of the fiber architecture between children and adults in dorsal and ventral visual streams. Hum Brain Mapp 32:935–946PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McCarty ME, Clifton RK, Ashmead DH, Lee P, Goubet N (2001) How infants use vision for grasping objects. Child Dev 72:973–987PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Morrongiello BA, Rocca PT (1989) Visual feedback and anticipatory hand orientation during infants’ reaching. Percept Mot Skill 69:787–802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Newell KM, Scully DM, McDonald PV, Baillargeon R (1989) Task constraints and infant grip configurations. Dev Psychobiol 22:817–831PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Newell KM, Scully DM, McDonald PV, Baillargeon R (1993) Body scale and infant grip configurations. Dev Psychobiol 26:195–205PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Perris E, Clifton R (1988) Reaching in the dark towards sound as a measure of auditory localization in infants. Infant Behav Dev 11:473–492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G, Matelli M (1998) The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 106:283–296PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Robin DJ, Berthier NE, Clifton RK (1996) Infants’ predictive reaching for moving objects in the dark. Dev Psychol 32:824–835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rönnqvist L, von Hofsten C (1994) Neonatal finger and arm movements as determined by a social and an object context. Early Dev Parent 3:81–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sacrey LA, Karl JM, Whishaw IQ (2012) Development of visual and somatosensory attention of the reach-to-eat movement in human infants aged 6 to 12. Exp Brain Res 223:121–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schum N, Jovanovic B, Schwarzer G (2011) Ten- and twelve-month-olds’ visual anticipation of orientation and size during grasping. J Exp Child Psychol 109:218–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Siddiqui A (1995) Object size as a determinant of grasping in infancy. J Genet Psychol 156:345–358PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tanné-Gariépy J, Rouiller EM, Boussaoud D (2002) Parietal inputs to dorsal versus ventral premotor areas in the macaque monkey: evidence for largely segregated visuomotor pathways. Exp Brain Res 145:91–103PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Thelen E, Spencer JP (1998) Postural control during reaching in young infants: a dynamic systems approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:507–514PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Thelen E, Corbetta D, Kamm K, Spencer JP, Schneider K, Zernicke RF (1993) The transition to reaching: mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Dev 64:1058–1098PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Thomas BL, Karl JM, Whishaw IQ. The origins of grasping in hand-to-body contacts in human infants during the first 6 months (in preparation)Google Scholar
  52. Twitchell TE (1965) The automatic grasping responses of infants. Neuropsychologia 3:247–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Van Der Fits IB, Hadders-Algra M (1998) The development of postural response patterns during reaching in healthy infants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:521–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. von Hofsten C (1982) Eye–hand coordination in the newborn. Dev Psychol 18:450–461CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. von Hofsten C (1984) Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements. Dev Psychol 3:378–388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. von Hofsten C (1991) Structuring of early reaching movements: a longitudinal study. J Mot Behav 23:280–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Von Hofsten C, Fazel-Zandy S (1984) Development of visually guided hand orientation in reaching. J Exp Child Psychol 38:208–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Von Hofsten C, Rönnqvist L (1988) Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. J Exp Psychol 14:610–621Google Scholar
  59. von Hofsten C, Rosander K (1997) Development of smooth pursuit tracking in young infants. Vision Res 37:1799–1810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wallace PS, Whishaw IQ (2003) Independent digit movements and precision grip patterns in 1–5-month-old human infants: hand-babbling, including vacuous then self-directed hand and digit movements, precedes targeted reaching. Neuropsychologia 4:1912–1918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wentworth N, Benson JB, Haith MM (2000) The development of infants’ reaches for stationary and moving objects. Child Dev 71:576–601PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Whishaw IQ, Suchowersky O, Davis L, Sarna J, Metz GA, Pellis SM (2002) Impairment of pronation, supination, and body co-ordination in reach-to-grasp tasks in human Parkinson’s disease (PD) reveals homology to deficits in animal models. Behav Brain Res 133:165–176PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. White LB, Castle P, Held R (1964) Observations on the development of visually-directed reaching. Child Dev 35:349–364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Witherington DC (2005) The development of prospective grasping control between 5 and 7 months: a longitudinal study. Infancy 7:143–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Neuroscience, Canadian Centre for Behavioural NeuroscienceUniversity of LethbridgeLethbridgeCanada

Personalised recommendations