Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 232, Issue 3, pp 855–864 | Cite as

Dissociating affordance and spatial compatibility effects using a pantomimed reaching action

  • Samuel Couth
  • Emma Gowen
  • Ellen Poliakoff
Research Article

Abstract

Previous research has demonstrated faster reaction times in response to appropriately oriented action-inducing stimuli (affordance effect, e.g. Tucker and Ellis in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:830–846, 1998). However, it has been argued that faster responses may be due to a spatial compatibility effect. In the current investigation, we aimed to dissociate the affordance and spatial compatibility effects. Moreover, we explored these effects beyond button-press responses by measuring detailed kinematics of the arms and hands during a naturalistic reach response. Participants were presented with images of a door handle (affording) or an abstract (non-affording) stimulus and made a pantomimed reach response with either hand depending on a colour change of the stimulus (i.e. Blue = left, Green = right). Stimuli could be aligned as spatially compatible or incompatible with the responding hand. The colour change occurred after a delay of 0, 500 or 1,000 ms. Only spatially compatible affordance stimuli facilitated reach onset compared to other stimuli and compatibility combinations, replicating previous reaction time studies. Therefore, in the absence of graspable stimuli, spatial compatibility alone was not sufficient to facilitate reach onset. There was also a larger outwards deviation of reach trajectory for spatially incompatible abstract stimuli compared to spatially compatible abstract stimuli, which waned with stimulus onset delay. However, no such affect was observed for the affording stimuli. Accordingly, later kinematics of the reaching action was influenced by the spatial compatibility of the stimulus alone. Overall, the dissociation of affordance and spatial compatibility effects suggests that these effects are driven by visuomotor priming and the inhibition of the incompatible spatial location, respectively.

Keywords

Spatial compatibility Affordance Kinematics Visuomotor 

Notes

Acknowledgments

SC was supported by the Frederick Craven Moore Award from the University of Manchester.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Anderson SJ, Yamagishi N, Karavia V (2002) Attentional processes link perception and action. Proc Biol Sci 269:1225–1232. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.1998 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bingham G, Coats R, Mon-Williams M (2007) Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed. Neuropsychologia 45:288–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.011 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bub DN, Masson ME (2010) Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:341–358. doi: 10.1037/a0017606 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Buhlmann I, Umiltà C, Wascher E (2007) Response coding and visuomotor transformation in the Simon task: the role of action goals. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:1269–1282. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1269 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ellis R, Tucker M (2000) Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. Br J Psychol 91(Pt 4):451–471PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Galpin A, Tipper SP, Dick JP, Poliakoff E (2011) Object affordance and spatial-compatibility effects in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 47:332–341. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.011 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gibson J (1977) The theory of affordances. In: Shaw R, Bransford J (eds) Perceiving, acting and knowing: toward an ecological psychology. Lawrnce Erlbraum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 67–82Google Scholar
  8. Glover S (2004) Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav Brain Sci 27:3–24 (discussion 24–78)PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Goodale MA, Jakobson LS, Keillor JM (1994) Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia 32:1159–1178PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Grèzes J, Tucker M, Armony J, Ellis R, Passingham RE (2003) Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing. Eur J Neurosci 17:2735–2740PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hasher L, Zacks RT (1988) Working memory, comprehension and aging: a review and new view. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp 193–225Google Scholar
  12. Hasher L, Stoltzfus ER, Zacks RT, Rypma B (1991) Age and inhibition. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 17:163–169PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hermsdörfer J, Terlinden G, Mühlau M, Goldenberg G, Wohlschläger AM (2007) Neural representations of pantomimed and actual tool use: evidence from an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 36(Suppl 2):T109–T118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.037 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hermsdörfer J, Li Y, Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Johannsen L (2012) Tool use without a tool: kinematic characteristics of pantomiming as compared to actual use and the effect of brain damage. Exp Brain Res 218:201–214. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3021-z PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hommel B (1994) Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychol Res 56:261–268PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Howard LA, Tipper SP (1997) Hand deviations away from visual cues: indirect evidence for inhibition. Exp Brain Res 113:144–152PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Iani C, Baroni G, Pellicano A, Nicoletti R (2011) On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: are the effects really independent. J Cogn Psychol 23:121–131. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.467251 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jeannerod M (1984) The timing of natural prehension movements. J Mot Behav 16:235–254PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jeannerod M (1994a) The hand and the object: the role of posterior parietal cortex in forming motor representations. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 72:535–541PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jeannerod M (1994b) The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav Brain Sci 17:187–202. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kornblum S, Stevens GT, Whipple A, Requin J (1999) The effects of irrelevant stimuli: 1. The time course of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response consistency effects with stroop-like stimuli, simon-like tasks, and their factorial combinations. J Exp Psychol 25:688–714Google Scholar
  22. Masson ME, Bub DN, Breuer AT (2011) Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 37:1470–1484. doi: 10.1037/a0023509 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Neyedli H, Welsh T (2012) The processes of facilitation and inhibition in a cue-target paradigm: insight from movement trajectory deviations. Acta Psychol (Amst) 139:159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.11.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pappas Z, Mack A (2008) Potentiation of action by undetected affordant objects. Vis Cogn 16:892–915CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pavese A, Buxbaum LJ (2002) Action matters: the role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action. Vis Cogn 9:559–590. doi: 10.1080/13506280143000584 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Persad CC, Abeles N, Zacks RT, Denburg NL (2002) Inhibitory changes after age 60 and their relationship to measures of attention and memory. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 57:P223–P232. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.3.P223 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Phillips JC, Ward R (2002) S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Vis Cogn 9:540–558. doi: 10.1080/13506280143000575 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Poliakoff E, Galpin A, Dick J, Moore P, Tipper SP (2007) The effect of viewing graspable objects and actions in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroReport 18:483–487. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32805867a1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Simon JR (1969) Reactions towards the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol 81:174–176PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Simon JR, Small AM (1969) Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. J Appl Psychol 53:433–435PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Simon JR, Acosta E, Mewaldt S, Speidel C (1976) The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing. Percept Psychophys 19:16–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Symes E, Ellis R, Tucker M (2005) Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Vis Cogn 12:1337–1361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Symes E, Ellis R, Tucker M (2007) Visual object affordances: object orientation. Acta Psychol (Amst) 124:238–255. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.03.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tipper SP, Lortie C, Baylis GC (1992) Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:891–905PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Tipper SP, Howard LA, Jackson SR (1997) Selective reaching to grasp: evidence for distractor interference effects. Vis Cogn 4:1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tipper SP, Paul MA, Hayes AE (2006) Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychon Bull Rev 13:493–498PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tucker M, Ellis R (1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:830–846PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. van Selst M, Jolicoeur P (1994) A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Q J Exp Psychol A 47:631–650. doi: 10.1080/14640749408401131 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wilf M, Holmes NP, Schwartz I, Makin TR (2013) Dissociating between object affordances and spatial compatibility effects using early response components. Front Psychol 4:591. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00591 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Life SciencesUniversity of ManchesterManchesterUK
  2. 2.School of Psychological SciencesUniversity of ManchesterManchesterUK

Personalised recommendations