Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 232, Issue 2, pp 481–491 | Cite as

Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom

Research Article

Abstract

Research on the experience and expression of boredom is underdeveloped. The purpose of the present study was to explore the psychophysiological signature of the subjective experience of boredom. Healthy undergraduates (n = 72) viewed previously validated and standardized video clips to induce boredom, sadness, and a neutral affective state, while their heart rate (HR), skin conductance levels (SCL), and cortisol levels were measured. Boredom yielded dynamic psychophysiological responses that differed from the other emotional states. Of particular interest, the physiological signature of boredom relative to sadness was characterized by rising HR, decreased SCL, and increased cortisol levels. This pattern of results suggests that boredom may be associated with both increased arousal and difficulties with sustained attention. These findings may help to resolve divergent conceptualizations of boredom in the extant literature and, ultimately, to enhance our understanding and treatment of clinical syndromes in which self-reported boredom is a prominent symptom.

Keywords

Boredom Depression Heart rate Skin conductance levels Attention 

References

  1. Barmack JE (1939) A definition of boredom: a reply to Mr. Berman. Am J Psychol 52:467–471Google Scholar
  2. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996) Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlyne DE (1960) Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. McGraw-Hill, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blaszczynski A, McConaghy N, Franknova A (1990) Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychol Rep 67:35–42PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Carriere JSA, Cheyne JA, Smilek D (2008) Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Conscious Cognit 17:835–847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheyne JA, Carriere JSA, Smilek D (2006) Absent-mindedness: lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Conscious Cognit 15:578–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Coles MGH (1972) Cardiac and respiratory activity during visual search. J Exp Psychol 96:371–379PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. de Weerth C, Graat G, Buitelaar JK, Thijssen JHH (2003) Measurement of cortisol in small quantities of saliva. Clin Chem 49:658–660PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eastwood JD, Frischen A, Fenske MJ, Smilek D (2013) The unengaged mind: defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect Psychol Sci 7:482–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Ancoli S (1980) Relative importance of face, body, and speech judgements of personality and affect. J Perspect Soc Psychol 38:270–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fahlman SA, Mercer KB, Gaskovski P, Eastwood AE, Eastwood JD (2009) Does a lack of meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal and experimental analyses. J Soc Clin Psychol 28:307–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fahlman SA, Mercer-Lynn KB, Flora DB, Eastwood JD (2013) Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment 20:68–85PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Farmer R, Sundberg ND (1986) Boredom proneness—the development and correlates of a new scale. J Perspect Assess 50:4–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fenichel H (1951) Review of the literature on boredom. Psychoanal Quart 20:345–346Google Scholar
  15. Fothergill A, (director), Berlowitz V, Malone S, Lemire M (producers) (2007) Planet earth [film]. Warner Home Video, BurbankGoogle Scholar
  16. Frith CD, Allen HA (1983) The skin conductance orienting response as an index of attention. Biol Psychol 17:27–39PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Giewitz PJ (1966) Structure of boredom. J Perspect Soc Psychol 3:592–600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Goldberg Y, Danckert J (2013) Traumatic brain injury, boredom and depression. Behav Sci 3:434–444CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Goldberg YK, Eastwood JD, La Guardia JG, Danckert J (2011) Boredom: an emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia or depression. J Soc Clin Psychol 30:647–666CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gross JJ, Levenson RW (1995) Emotion elicitation using films. Cognit Emot 9:87–108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hebb DO (1955) Drives and the CNS (conceptual nervous system). Psychol Rev 62:243–254PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hermens DF, Williams LM, Lazzaro I, Whitmont S, Melkonian D, Gordon E (2004) Sex differences in adult ADHD: a double dissociation in brain activity and autonomic arousal. Biol Psychol 66:221–233PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hofmann SG, Moscovitch DA, Kim H-J (2005) Autonomic correlates of social anxiety and embarrassment in shy and non-shy individuals. Int J Psychophysiol 61:134–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH (1989) Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: an overview. Neuropsychobiology 22:150–169PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lacey JI (1959) Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. In: Rubinstein EA, Parloff MB (eds) Research in psychotherapy. American Psychological Association, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  26. Lacey JI, Lacey BC (1970) Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In: Black P (ed) Physiological correlates of emotion. Academic Press, New York, pp 205–228Google Scholar
  27. Lackschewitz H, Huther G, Kroner-Herwig B (2008) Physiological and psychological stress responses in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychoneuroendocrinology 33:612–624PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. LeDoux I (1996) The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon and Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  29. Lee CM, Neighbors C, Woods BA (2007) Marijuana motives: young adults’ reasons for using marijuana. Addict Behav 32:1384–1394PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. LePera N (2011) Relationships between boredom proneness, mindfulness, anxiety, depression, and substance use. New Sch Psychol Bull 8:15–25Google Scholar
  31. London H, Schubert DS, Washburn D (1972) Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. J Abnorm Psychol 80:29–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lovell D, (producer), Zeffirelli F (director) (1979) The Champ [film]. MGM/Pathe Home Video, Culver CityGoogle Scholar
  33. Lundberg U, Melin B, Gary WE, Holmberg L (1993) Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: learning versus repetitive data entry. Ergonomics 36:601–611PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Malkovsky E, Merrifield C, Goldberg YK, Danckert J (2012) Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Exp Brain Res 221:59–67PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Martin M, Sadlo G, Stew G (2006) The phenomenon of boredom. Qual Res Psychol 3:193–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mercer KB, Eastwood JD (2010) Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by “boredom”. Int Gambl Stud 10:91–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mikulas WL, Vodanovich SJ (1993) The essence of boredom. Psychol. Record 43:3–12Google Scholar
  38. O’Connell RG, Bellgrove MA, Dockree PM, Lau A, Fitzgerald M, Robertson IH (2008) Self-alert training: volitional modulation of autonomic arousal improves sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 46:1379–1390PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Obrist PA, Webb RA, Sutterer JR, Howard JL (1970) Cardiac deceleration and reaction time: an evaluation of two hypotheses. Psychophysics 6:695–706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Öhman A, Hamm A, Hugdahl K (2000) Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: orienting anticipation, and conditioning. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (eds) Handbook of psychophysiology, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Ohsuga M, Shimono F, Genno H (2001) Assessment of phasic work stress using autonomic indices. Int J Psychophys 40:211–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Papillo JF, Shapiro D (1990) The cardiovascular system. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG (eds) Principles of psychophysiology: physical, social, and inferential elements. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 456–512Google Scholar
  43. Passik SD (2003) Boredom as a component of distress in advanced cancer patients. Psycho-Oncology 12:227Google Scholar
  44. Passik SD, Inman A, Kirsh K, Theobald DE, Dickerson P (2003) Initial validation of a scale to measure purposelessness, understimulation, and boredom in cancer patients: toward a redefinition of depression in advanced disease. Palliat Support Care 1:41–50PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E (2008) Psychophysiological investigation of the vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? Phys Behav 93:369–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Philippot P (1993) Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognit Emot 7:171–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Seel RT, Kreutzer JS (2003) Depression assessment after traumatic brain injury: an empirically based classification method. Arch Phys Med Rehab 84:1621–1628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sommers J, Vodanovich SJ (2000) Boredom proneness: its relationship to psychological- and physical-health symptoms. J Clin Psychol 56:149–155PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Strahler J, Mueller A, Rosenloecher F, Kirschbaum C, Rohleder N (2010) Salivary α-amylase stress reactivity across different age groups. Psychophysics 47:587–595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Theobald DE, Kirsh KL, Holtsclaw E, Donaghy K, Passik SD (2003) An open label pilot study of citalopram for depression and boredom in ambulatory cancer patients. Palliat Support Care 1:71–77PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Van Tilberg WAP, Igou ER (2012) On boredom: lack of challenge as distinct boredom experiences. Motiv Emot 36:181–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Vodanovich SJ (2003) Psychometric measures of boredom: a review of the literature. J Psychol Interdiscip Appl 137:569–595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Vodanovich SJ, Verner KM, Gilbride TV (1991) Boredom proneness: its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychol Rep 69:1139–1146PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS Scales. J Perspect Soc Psychol 47:1063–1070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Watt JD, Vodanovich SJ (1992) Relationship between boredom proneness and impulsivity. Psychol Rep 70:688–690PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WaterlooWaterlooCanada

Personalised recommendations