Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 227, Issue 3, pp 397–405 | Cite as

Differing effects of prosaccades and antisaccades on postural stability

  • Agathe LegrandEmail author
  • Karine Doré Mazars
  • Julie Lazzareschi
  • Christelle Lemoine
  • Isabelle Olivier
  • Julien Barra
  • Maria Pia Bucci
Research Article

Abstract

The goal of the study was to examine the effect of different types of eye movements on postural stability. Ten healthy young adults (25 ± 3 years) participated in the study. Postural control was measured by the TechnoConcept© platform and recorded in Standard Romberg and Tandem Romberg conditions while participants performed five oculomotor tasks: two fixation tasks (central fixation cross, without and with distractors), two prosaccade tasks toward peripheral targets displayed 4° to the left or to the right of the fixation cross (reactive saccades induced by a gap 0 ms paradigm and voluntary saccades induced by an overlap 600 ms paradigm) and one antisaccade task (voluntary saccade made in the opposite direction of the visual target). The surface, the length, and the mean speed of the center of pressure were analyzed. We found that saccadic eye movements improved postural stability with respect to the fixation tasks. Furthermore, antisaccades were found to decrease postural stability compared to prosaccades (reactive as well as voluntary saccades). This result is in line with the U-shaped nonlinear model described by Lacour et al. (Neurophysiol Clin 38:411–421, 2008), showing that a secondary task performed during a postural task could increase (prosaccade task) or decrease (antisacade task) postural stability depending on its complexity. We suggest that the different degree of attentional resources needed for performing prosaccade or antisaccade tasks are, most likely, responsible for the different effect on postural control.

Keywords

Posture Eye movements Antisaccades Prosaccades Dual-task 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Agathe Legrand was supported by the prize of “Association Posture et Equilibre,” Grenoble 2010.

Conflict of interest

Authors have no financial and personal relationships with other people or organizations that could inappropriately influence or bias their work in this study.

References

  1. Barra J, Bray A, Sahni V, Golding JF, Gresty MA (2006) Increasing cognitive load with increasing balance challenge: recipe for catastrophe. Exp Brain Res 174(4):734–745PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beilock SL, Carr TH, MacMahon C, Starkes JL (2002) When paying attention becomes counter productive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. J Exp Psychol Appl 8:6–16PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2009) When are attention and saccade preparation dissociated? Psychol Sci 20(11):1340–1347PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blanchard Y, Carey S, Coffey J, Cohen A, Harris T, Michlik S, Pellecchia GL (2005) The influence of concurrent cognitive tasks on postural sway in children. Pediatr Phys Ther Fall 17(3):189–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bove M, Fenoggio C, Tacchino A, Pelosin E, Schieppati M (2009) Interaction between vision and neck proprioception in the control of stance. Neuroscience 164:1601–1608PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chiari L, Rocchi L, Capello A (2009) Stabilometric parameters are affected by anthropometry and foot placement. Clin Biomech 17:666–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clementz BA, Brahmbhatt SB, McDowell JE, Brown R, Sweeney JA (2007) When does the brain inform the eyes whether and where to move? An EEG study in humans. Cereb Cortex 17(11):2634–2643PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Deubel H, Schneider W (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Res 36(12):1827–1837PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ettinger U, Ffytche DH, Kumari V, Kathmann N, Reuter B, Zelaya F et al (2008) Decomposing the neural correlates of anti-saccade eye movements using event-related FMRI. Cereb Cortex 18(5):1148–1159PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Everling S, Munoz DP (2000) Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurosci 20(1):387–400PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Fischer B (1987) The preparation of visually guided saccades. Rev Physiol Biochem Pharmacol 106:1–35PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gagey PM, Weber B (1999) Posturology; Regulation and disruption of standing, 2nd edn. Masson, ParisGoogle Scholar
  13. Geurts AC, Mulder TW, Nienhuis B, Rijken RA (1991) Dual-task assessment of reorganization of postural control in persons with lower limb amputation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 72:1059–1064PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Geurts AC, Nienhuis B, Mulder TW (1993) Intrasubject variability of selected force platform parameters in the quantification of postural control. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 74:1144–1150PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Glasauer S, Schneider E, Jahn K, Strupp M, Brandt T (2005) How the eyes move the body. Neurology 65(8):1291–1293 (Epub 2005 Jul 28)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grasso R, Ivanenko Y, Lacquaniti F (1999) Time course of gaze influences on postural responses to neck proprioceptive and galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans. Neurosci Lett 273:121–124PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hallet P (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res 18:1279–1296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Horak FB, Macpherson JM (1996) Balance orientation and equilibrium. In: Shepard J, Rowell L (eds) Handbook of physiology. Exercise: Regulation and integration of multiple systems, section 12, ch. 7. Oxford University Press, NY, pp 255–292Google Scholar
  19. Huang HJ, Mercer VS (2001) Dual-task methodology: applications in studies of cognitive and motor performance in adults and children. Pediatric Physical Therapy 13:133–140PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hunter MC, Hoffman MA (2001) Postural control: visual and cognitive manipulations. Gait Posture 13:41–48PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Huxhold O, Li S-C, Schmiedek F, Lindenberger U (2006) Dual-tasking postural control: aging and the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attention. Brain Res Bull 69:294–305PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kerr B, Condon SM, McDonald LA (1985) Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture. J Exp Psychol 11:617–622Google Scholar
  23. Lacour M, Bernard-Demanze L, Dumitrescu M (2008) Posture control, aging, and attention resources: models and posture-analysis methods. Neurophysiol Clin 38:411–421PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lajoie Y, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M (1993) Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium. Exp Brain Res 97:139–144PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Leigh RJ, Zee DS (2006) The neurology of eye movement, 4th edn. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  26. Maki B, Holliday P, Fernie G (1990) Aging and postural control: a comparison of spontaneous- and induced-way balance tests. J Am Geriatr Soc 38:1–9PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Matsuda T, Matsuura M, Ohkubo T, Ohkubo H, Matsushima E, Inoue K et al (2004) Functional MRI mapping of brain activation during visually guided saccades and anti-saccades: cortical and subcortical networks. Psychiatry Res 131(2):147–155PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. McNevin NH, Wulf G (2000) Carlson C effects of attentional focus, self-control, and dyad training on motor learning: implications for physical rehabilitation. Phys Therapy 80:373–385Google Scholar
  29. Melzer I, Benjuya N, Kaplanski J (2001) Age-related changes of postural control: effect of cognitive tasks. Gerontology 47:189–194PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Olivier I, Cuisinier C, Vaugoyeau M, Nougier V, Assaiante C (2007) Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference in 7-year-olds and adults. NeuroReport 18(8):817–821PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Olivier I, Palluel E, Nougier V (2008) Effects of attentional focus on postural sway in children and adults. Exp Brain Res 185:341–345PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Palluel E, Nougier V, Olivier I (2010) Postural control and attentional demand during adolescence. Brain Res 1358:151–159 (Epub 2010 Aug 22)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rougier P, Garin M (2006) Performing saccadic eye movements modifies postural control organization. Neurophysiol Clin 36(4):235–243PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rougier P, Garin M (2007) Performing saccadic eye movements or blinking improves postural control. Mot Control 11(3):213–223Google Scholar
  35. Schaefer S, Lövdén M, Wieckhorst B, Lindenberger U (2010) Cognitive performance is improved while walking: differences in cognitive–sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. Eur J Dev Psychol 7(3):371–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M (2000) Attentional demands and postural control: the effect of sensory context. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55(1):10–16Google Scholar
  37. Stoffregen TA, Bardy BG, Bonnet CT, Hove P, Oullier O (2007) Postural sway and the frequency of horizontal eye movements. Mot Control 11(1):86–102Google Scholar
  38. Straube A, Paulus W, Quintern J, Brandt T (1989) Visual ataxia induced by eye movements: posturographic measurements in normal and patients with ocular motor disorders. Clin Vis Sci 4(2):107–113Google Scholar
  39. Uchida T, Hashimoto M, Suzuki N, Takegami T, Iwase I (1979) Effects of periodic saccades on the body sway in human subjects. Neurosci Lett 13:253–258PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Vuillerme N, Nafati G (2007) How attentional focus on body sway affects postural control during quiet standing? Psychol Res 71(2):192–200PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Vuillerme N, Chenu O, Pinsault N, Fleury A, Demongeot J (2008) Can a plantar pressure-based tongue-placed electrotactile biofeedback improve postural control under altered vestibular and neck proprioceptive conditions? Neuroscience 155:291–296PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. White KD, Post RB, Leibowitz HW (1980) Saccadic eye movements and body sway. Science 208(4444):621–623PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wickens CD (1974) Temporal limits of human information processing: a developmental study. Psychol Bul 81:739–755CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Winter DA (1995) Human balance and posture control during standing and walking. Gait Posture 3:193–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A (2002) Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16(1):1–14PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wulf G, Prinz W (2001) Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: a review. Psychon Bull Rev 8:648–660PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Agathe Legrand
    • 1
    • 4
    Email author
  • Karine Doré Mazars
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
  • Julie Lazzareschi
    • 1
  • Christelle Lemoine
    • 1
    • 4
  • Isabelle Olivier
    • 3
  • Julien Barra
    • 1
    • 6
  • Maria Pia Bucci
    • 1
    • 5
  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neuropsychologie Cognitives, FRE 3292 CNRS, IUPDPUniversité Paris DescartesBoulogne-BillancourtFrance
  2. 2.Institut Universitaire de FranceParis France
  3. 3.Laboratoire TIMC-IMAG, UMR CNRS 5525, Faculté de médecineUniversité Joseph FourierLa TroncheFrance
  4. 4.Laboratoire Vision Action Cognition EAU01, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Institut de Psychologie, IUPDPUniversité Paris DescartesBoulogne-BillancourtFrance
  5. 5.Laboratoire de Physiopathologie et neuroprotection des atteintes du cerveau en développement UMR 676 INSERM, Hôpital Robert DebréUniversité Paris DiderotParisFrance
  6. 6.Laboratoire Mémoire Cognition EAU02, Institut de Psychologie, IUPDPUniversité Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris CitéBoulogne-BillancourtFrance

Personalised recommendations