Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 229, Issue 3, pp 301–312 | Cite as

Imaging volition: what the brain can tell us about the will

  • Marcel Brass
  • Margaret T. Lynn
  • Jelle Demanet
  • Davide Rigoni
Volition

Abstract

The question of how we can voluntarily control our behaviour dates back to the beginnings of scientific psychology. Currently, there are two empirical research disciplines tackling human volition: cognitive neuroscience and social psychology. To date, there is little interaction between the two disciplines in terms of the investigation of human volition. The aim of the current article is to highlight recent brain imaging work on human volition and to relate social psychological concepts of volition to the functional neuroanatomy of intentional action. A host of studies indicate that the medial prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role in voluntary action. Accordingly, we postulate that social psychological concepts of volition can be investigated using neuroimaging techniques, and propose that by developing a social cognitive neuroscience of human volition, we may gain a deeper understanding of this fascinating and complex aspect of the human mind.

Keywords

Volition Intentional control Medial prefrontal cortex fMRI 

References

  1. Abramson LY, Seligman ME, Teasdale JD (1978) Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol 87:49–74PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baumeister RF (2003) Ego depletion and self-regulation failure: a resource model of self-control. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27:281–284PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baumeister RF (2008) Free will in scientific psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci 3:14–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM (1998) Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol 74:1252–1265PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumeister RF, Masicampo EJ, Dewall CN (2009) Prosocial benefits of feeling free: disbelief in free will increases aggression and reduces helpfulness. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35:260–268PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beedie CJ, Lane AM (2011) The role of glucose in self-control: another look at the evidence and an alternative conceptualization. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 16:143–153PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berman BD, Horovitz SG, Morel B, Hallett M (2012) Neural correlates of blink suppression and the buildup of a natural bodily urge. Neuroimage 59:1441–1450PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bjork JM, Hommer DW (2007) Anticipating instrumentally obtained and passively-received rewards: a factorial fMRI investigation. Behav Brain Res 177:165–170PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bode S, He AH, Soon CS, Trampel R, Turner R, Haynes JD (2011) Tracking the unconscious generation of free decisions using ultra-high field fMRI. PLoS One 6:e21612PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bohlhalter S, Goldfine A, Matteson S, Garraux G, Hanakawa T, Kansaku K, Wurzman R, Hallett M (2006) Neural correlates of tic generation in Tourette syndrome: an event-related functional MRI study. Brain 129:2029–2037PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boucher HC, Kofos MN (2012) The idea of money counteracts ego depletion effects. J Exp Soc Psychol 48:804–810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brass M, Haggard P (2007) To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. J Neurosci 27:9141–9145PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brass M, Haggard P (2008) The what, when, whether model of intentional action. Neuroscientist 14:319–325PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brass M, Haggard P (2010) The hidden side of intentional action: the role of the anterior insular cortex. Brain Struct Funct 214:603–610PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1124:1–38PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chung JY, Yoon HW, Song MS, Park H (2006) Event related fMRI studies of voluntary and inhibited eye blinking using a time marker of EOG. Neurosci Lett 395:196–200PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clarkson JJ, Hirt ER, Jia L, Alexander MB (2010) When perception is more than reality: the effects of perceived versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 98:29–46PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Craig AD (2009) How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:59–70PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cunnington R, Windischberger C, Deecke L, Moser E (2002) The preparation and execution of self-initiated and externally-triggered movement: a study of event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 15:373–385PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cunnington R, Windischberger C, Deecke L, Moser E (2003) The preparation and readiness for voluntary movement: a high-field event-related fMRI study of the Bereitschafts-BOLD response. Neuroimage 20:404–412PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cunnington R, Windischberger C, Robinson S, Moser E (2006) The selection of intended actions and the observation of others’ actions: a time-resolved fMRI study. Neuroimage 29:1294–1302PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Custers R, Aarts H (2010) The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science 329:47–50PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. De Pesapia N, Turatto M, Lin P, Jovicich J, Caramazza A (2012) Unconscious priming instructions modulate activity in default and executive networks of the human brain. Cereb Cortex 22:639–649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Decety J, Lamm C (2007) The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist 13:580–593PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Demanet J, De Baene W, Arrington CM, Brass M (2013) Biasing free choices: the role of the RCZ in intentional control. Neuroimage 72:207–213Google Scholar
  26. Desmurget M, Sirigu A (2009) A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:411–419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Desmurget M, Reilly KT, Richard N, Szathmari A, Mottolese C, Sirigu A (2009) Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324:811–813PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dijksterhuis A, Aarts H (2010) Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annu Rev Psychol 61:467–490PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Dijksterhuis A, Bargh JA (2001) The perception-behavior expressway: automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 33:1–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB (2006) On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311:1005–1007PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Elsner B, Hommel B (2001) Effect anticipation and action control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:229–240PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Farrer C, Frith CD (2002) Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage 15:596–603PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Farrer C, Franck N, Georgieff N, Frith CD, Decety J, Jeannerod M (2003) Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage 18:324–333PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Filevich E, Kuhn S, Haggard P (2012) Intentional inhibition in human action: the power of ‘no’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1107–1118PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Freud S (1923) Das Ich und das Es. Internationaler Psycho-analytischer Verlag, LeipzigGoogle Scholar
  36. Fried I, Katz A, McCarthy G, Sass KJ, Williamson P, Spencer SS, Spencer DD (1991) Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. J Neurosci 11:3656–3666PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF (2007) The physiology of willpower: linking blood glucose to self-control. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 11:303–327PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, Maner JK, Plant EA, Tice DM, Brewer LE, Schmeichel BJ (2007) Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:325–336PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Gallagher II (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4:14–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gilbert SJ (2011) Decoding the content of delayed intentions. J Neurosci 31:2888–2894PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gilbert SJ, Gollwitzer PM, Cohen AL, Burgess PW, Oettingen G (2009) Separable brain systems supporting cued versus self-initiated realization of delayed intentions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35:905–915PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Goldberg G (1985) Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci 8:567–616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gollwitzer PM (1999) Implementation intentions: strong effects with simple plans. Am Psychol 54:493–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Haggard P (2008) Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:934–946PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hallett M (2007) Volitional control of movement: the physiology of free will. Clin Neurophysiol 118:1179–1192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Hartstra E, Kuhn S, Verguts T, Brass M (2011) The implementation of verbal instructions: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 32:1811–1824PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Hartstra E, Waszak F, Brass M (2012) The implementation of verbal instructions: dissociating motor preparation from the formation of stimulus-response associations. Neuroimage 63(3):1143–1153Google Scholar
  48. Haynes JD (2011) Decoding and predicting intentions. Ann NY Acad Sci 1224:9–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Heckhausen J (2007) The motivation-volition divide and its resolution in action-phase models of behavioral and developmental regulation. Res Hum Dev 4:163–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Hoffstaedter F, Grefkes C, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB (2013) The “what” and “when” of self-initiated movements. Cereb Cortex 23(3):520–530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr391 Google Scholar
  51. Holland RW, Hendriks M, Aarts H (2005) Smells like clean spirit. Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychol Sci 16:689–693PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Holroyd CB, Yeung N (2012) Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 16:122–128PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Jackson SR, Parkinson A, Kim SY, Schuermann M, Eickhoff SB (2011) On the functional anatomy of the urge-for-action. Cogn Neurosci 2:227–243PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Jahanshahi M, Jenkins IH, Brown RG, Marsden CD, Passingham RE, Brooks DJ (1995) Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson’s disease subjects. Brain 118(Pt 4):913–933PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. James W (1890) The principles of psychology. MacMillan, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Job V, Dweck CS, Walton GM (2010) Ego depletion—is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychol Sci 21:1686–1693PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Johnson SC, Schmitz TW, Kawahara-Baccus TN, Rowley HA, Alexander AL, Lee J, Davidson RJ (2005) The cerebral response during subjective choice with and without self-reference. J Cogn Neurosci 17:1897–1906PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Kane R (ed) (1996) The significance of free will. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  59. Kay AC, Wheeler SC, Bargh JA, Ross L (2004) Material priming: the influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 95:83–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J (1999) The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature 399:148–151PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Krieghoff V, Brass M, Prinz W, Waszak F (2009) Dissociating what and when of intentional actions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3:1–10Google Scholar
  62. Krieghoff V, Waszak F, Prinz W, Brass M (2011) Neural and behavioral correlates of intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 49(5):767–776Google Scholar
  63. Kuhn S, Haggard P, Brass M (2009) Intentional inhibition: how the “veto-area” exerts control. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2834–2843PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Kuhn S, Gallinat J, Brass M (2011) “Keep calm and carry on”: structural correlates of expressive suppression of emotions. PLoS One 6:e16569PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Langer EJ (1975) The illusion of control. J Pers Soc Psychol 32:311–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Lau HC, Passingham RE (2007) Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 27:5805–5811PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Lau HC, Rogers RD, Haggard P, Passingham RE (2004a) Attention to intention. Science 303:1208–1210PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Lau HC, Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Passingham RE (2004b) Willed action and attention to the selection of action. Neuroimage 21:1407–1415PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Lau H, Rogers RD, Passingham RE (2006) Dissociating response selection and conflict in the medial frontal surface. Neuroimage 29:446–451PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Leotti LA, Iyengar SS, Ochsner KN (2010) Born to choose: the origins and value of the need for control. Trends Cogn Sci 14:457–463PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Lerner A, Bagic A, Hanakawa T, Boudreau EA, Pagan F, Mari Z, Bara-Jimenez W, Aksu M, Sato S, Murphy DL, Hallett M (2009) Involvement of insula and cingulate cortices in control and suppression of natural urges. Cereb Cortex 19:218–223PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Levy R, Dubois B (2006) Apathy and the functional anatomy of the prefrontal cortex-basal ganglia circuits. Cereb Cortex 16:916–928PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106(Pt 3):623–642PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:276–291PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Marsh R, Zhu HT, Wang ZS, Skudlarski P, Peterson BS (2007) A developmental fMRI study of self-regulatory control in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 164:955–966PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315:393–395PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Mazzone L, Shan Y, Blair C, Gunter BC, Wang ZS, Marsh R, Peterson BS (2010) An fMRI study of frontostriatal circuits during the inhibition of eye blinking in persons with Tourette syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 167:341–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Mele A (2009) Effective intentions: the power of conscious will. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Momennejad I, Haynes JD (2012) Human anterior prefrontal cortex encodes the ‘what’ and ‘when’ of future intentions. Neuroimage 61:139–148PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Mueller VA, Brass M, Waszak F, Prinz W (2007) The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions. Neuroimage 37:1354–1361PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Muraven M, Baumeister RF (2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol Bull 126:247–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Nachev P, Husain M (2010) Action and the fallacy of the ‘internal’: comment on Passingham et al. Trends Cogn Sci 14:192–193; author reply 193–194Google Scholar
  83. Obhi SS (2012) The troublesome distinction between self-generated and externally triggered action: a commentary on Schuur and Haggard. Conscious Cogn 21:587–588PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Obhi SS, Matkovich S, Chen R (2009) Changing the “when” and “what” of intended actions. J Neurophysiol 102:2755–2762PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. O’Doherty J, Critchley H, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ (2003) Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci 23:7931–7939PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, Dolan RJ (2004) Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304:452–454PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Pacherie E (2008) The phenomenology of action: a conceptual framework. Cognition 107:179–217PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Passingham RE, Bengtsson SL, Lau HC (2010) Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one’s own performance. Trends Cogn Sci 14:16–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Peterson BS, Skudlarski P, Anderson AW, Zhang HP, Gatenby C, Lacadie CM, Leckman JF, Gore JC (1998) A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of tic suppression in Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:326–333PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Reuss H, Kiesel A, Kunde W, Hommel B (2011) Unconscious activation of task sets. Conscious Cogn 20:556–567PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306:443–447PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Rigoni D, Kuhn S, Sartori G, Brass M (2011) Inducing disbelief in free will alters brain correlates of preconscious motor preparation: the brain minds whether we believe in free will or not. Psychol Sci 22:613–618PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Rigoni D, Kuhn S, Gaudino G, Sartori G, Brass M (2012) Reducing self-control by weakening free will beliefs. Conscious Cogn 21(3):1482–1490Google Scholar
  94. Roskies AL (2010) How does neuroscience affect our conception of volition? Annu Rev Neurosci 33:109–130PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Ruby P, Decety J (2001) Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat Neurosci 4:546–550PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Ruge H, Wolfensteller U (2010) Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. Cereb Cortex 20:1656–1667PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Schlaghecken F, Eimer M (2004) Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives. Psychon Bull Rev 11:463–468PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Schmitz TW, Johnson SC (2007) Relevance to self: a brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:585–596PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Schurger A, Sitt JD, Dehaene S (2012) An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(42):E2904–E2913. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210467109 Google Scholar
  100. Schuur F, Haggard P (2011) What are self-generated actions? Conscious Cogn 20:1697–1704PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Schuur F, Haggard P (2012) On capturing the essence of self-generated action: a reply to Obhi (2012). Conscious Cogn 21:1070–1071PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Searle JR (ed) (2004) Mind: a brief introduction. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  103. Shapiro DH Jr, Schwartz CE, Astin JA (1996) Controlling ourselves, controlling our world. Psychology’s role in understanding positive and negative consequences of seeking and gaining control. Am Psychol 51:1213–1230PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Soon CS, Brass M, Heinze HJ, Haynes JD (2008) Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nat Neurosci 11:543–545PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Spence SA, Brooks DJ, Hirsch SR, Liddle PF, Meehan J, Grasby PM (1997) A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain 120(Pt 11):1997–2011PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Spengler S, von Cramon DY, Brass M (2009) Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 46:290–298PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Stern E, Silbersweig DA, Chee KY, Holmes A, Robertson MM, Trimble M, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (2000) A functional neuroanatomy of ties in Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:741–748PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Strick M, Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Sjoerdsma A, van Baaren RB, Nordgren LF (2011) A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Soc Cogn 29:738–762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Tanaka SC, Balleine BW, O’Doherty JP (2008) Calculating consequences: brain systems that encode the causal effects of actions. J Neurosci 28:6750–6755PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Tricomi EM, Delgado MR, Fiez JA (2004) Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron 41:281–292PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. van Eimeren T, Wolbers T, Munchau A, Buchel C, Weiller C, Siebner HR (2006) Implementation of visuospatial cues in response selection. Neuroimage 29:286–294PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Vohs KD, Schooler JW (2008a) The value of believing in free will—encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychol Sci 19:49–54PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Vohs KD, Schooler JW (2008b) The value of believing in free will: encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychol Sci 19:49–54PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, Schmeichel BJ, Twenge JM, Nelson NM, Tice DM (2008) Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. J Pers Soc Psychol 94:883–898PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Wagner DD, Heatherton TF (2012) Self-regulatory depletion increases emotional reactivity in the amygdala. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Epub ahead of print]Google Scholar
  116. Walton ME, Devlin JT, Rushworth MF (2004) Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 7:1259–1265PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Wegner DM (2002) The illusion of conscious will. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  118. Wenke D, Fleming SM, Haggard P (2010) Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action. Cognition 115:26–38PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Filevich E, Vanneste P, Brass M, Fias W, Haggard P, Kühn S (in prep) Brain correlates of subjective freedom of choiceGoogle Scholar
  120. Zysset S, Huber O, Ferstl E, von Cramon DY (2002) The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 15:983–991PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marcel Brass
    • 1
    • 2
  • Margaret T. Lynn
    • 1
  • Jelle Demanet
    • 1
  • Davide Rigoni
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium
  2. 2.Behavioural Science InstituteRadboud UniversityNijmegenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations