Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 224, Issue 4, pp 647–655 | Cite as

Dissecting the response in response–effect compatibility

Research Article

Abstract

Voluntary actions are guided by sensory anticipations of body-related as well as environment-related action effects. Even though action effects in the environment typically resemble the action goal proper, anticipations of body-related effects can cause interference if they do not correspond to intended environment-related effects. The present study explored which specific response features cause such interference: the spatial location of the moving limb or its anatomical connection to the body causes such interference? Using a response–effect compatibility design with normal and crossed hand-key mappings, we show that environment-related effects are predominantly related to spatial rather than anatomical response features, ensuring that goal-directed behavior is flexible and efficient at the same time. Furthermore, results indicate that this mechanism applies to both, free- and forced-choice actions.

Keywords

Action control Effect anticipations Spatial versus anatomical codes Ideomotor theory 

References

  1. Ansorge U (2002) Spatial intention–response compatibility. Acta Psychol 109:285–299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ansorge U, Wühr P (2004) A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. J Exp Psychol Hum 30:365–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlucchi G, Crea F, Di Stefano M, Tassinari G (1977) Influence of spatial stimulus-response compatibility on reaction time of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized light stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum 3:505–517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Butz MV, Herbort O, Hoffmann J (2007) Exploiting redundancy for flexible behavior: unsupervised learning in a modular sensorimotor control architecture. Psychol Rev 114:1015–1046CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Conde E, Jazenko F, Fraga Filho RS, Harth da Costa D, Torro-Alves N, Cavallet M, Gawryszewski LG (2011) Stimulus affective valence reverses spatial compatibility effect. Psychol Neurosci 4:81–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dittrich K, Rothe A, Klauer KC (2012) Increased spatial salience in the social Simon task: a response-coding account of spatial compatibility effects. Atten Percept Psychophys 74:911–929CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dolk T, Hommel B, Colzato LS, Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W, Liepelt R (2011) How “social” is the social Simon effect? Front Psychol 2:1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Elsner B, Hommel B (2001) Effect anticipation and action control. J Exp Psychol Hum 27:229–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Guagnano D, Rusconia E, Umiltà CA (2010) Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition 114:348–355CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Heister G, Schröder-Heister P, Ehrenstein WH (1990) Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In: Proctor RW, Reeve TG (eds) Stimulus-response compatibility: an integrated perspective. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 117–143Google Scholar
  11. Herwig A, Horstmann G (2011) Action-effect associations revealed by eye movements. Psychon Bull Rev 18:531–537CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Herwig A, Prinz W, Waszak F (2007) Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Q J Exp Psychol 60:1540–1554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hoffmann J, Lenhard A, Sebald A, Pfister R (2009) Movements or targets: what makes an action in action effect learning? Q J Exp Psychol 62:2433–2449CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hommel B (1993) Inverting the Simon effect by intention. Psychol Res 55:270–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hommel B (1996) The cognitive representation of action: automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychol Res 59:176–186CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Hommel B (2004) Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends Cogn Sci 8:494–500CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hommel B (2011) The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychol 136:189–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–937CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Hommel B, Proctor RW, Vu KPL (2004) A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychol Res 68:1–17CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. James W (1890/1981) The principles of psychology. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  21. Janczyk M, Skirde S, Weigelt M, Kunde W (2009) Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. Hum Mov Sci 28:437–449CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Janczyk M, Pfister R, Kunde W (2012a) On the persistence of tool-based compatibility effects. J Psychol 220:16–22Google Scholar
  23. Janczyk M, Pfister R, Crognale MA, Kunde W (2012b) Effective rotations: action-effects determine the interplay of mental and manual rotations. J Exp Psychol Gen 141:489–501CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2004) Variable action effects: response control by context-specific effect anticipations. Psychol Res 68:155–162CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Klapp ST, Greim DM, Mendicino CM, Koenig RS (1979) Anatomic and environmental dimensions of stimulus-response compatibility: implication for theories of memory coding. Acta Psychol 43:367–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kunde W (2001) Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. J Exp Psychol Hum 27:387–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kunde W, Wühr P (2004) Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychol Res 68:199–207PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Kunde W, Koch I, Hoffmann J (2004) Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. Q J Exp Psychol A 57:87–106PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kunde W, Müsseler J, Heuer H (2007) Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Hum Factors 49:661–670CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kunde W, Lozo L, Neumann R (2011) Effect-based control of facial expressions. Evidence from action-effect compatibility. Psychon Bull Rev 18:820–826CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Mechsner F, Knoblich G (2004) Do muscles matter for coordinated action? J Exp Psychol Hum 30:490–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2001) Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature 414:69–73CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Memelink J, Hommel B (2012) Intentional weighting: a basic principle in cognitive control. Psychol Res. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y
  34. Metzker M, Dreisbach G (2009) Bidirectional priming processes in the Simon task. J Exp Psychol Hum 6:1770–1783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Müsseler J, Hommel B (1997) Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum 23:861–872CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Müsseler J, Skottke EM (2011) Compatibility relations with simple lever tools. Hum Factors 53:383–390CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Pfister R, Kiesel A, Melcher T (2010) Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychol 135:316–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pfister R, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2011) Learning at any rate: action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychol Res 75:61–65CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Pfister R, Heinemann A, Kiesel A, Thomaschke R, Janczyk M (2012) Do endogenous and exogenous action control compete for perception? J Exp Psychol Hum 38:279–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rieger M (2007) Letters as visual action-effects in skilled typing. Acta Psychol 126:138–153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Riggio L, Gawryszewski LG, Umiltá C (1986) What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? Acta Psychol 62:89–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shin YK, Proctor RW, Capaldi EJ (2010) A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychol Bull 136:943–974CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Simon JR, Rudell AP (1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. J Appl Psychol 51:300–304CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Simon JR, Hinrichs JV, Craft JL (1970) Auditory S-R compatibility: reaction time as a function of ear–hand correspondence and ear–response–location correspondence. J Exp Psychol 86:97–102CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Ticini LF, Schütz-Bosbach S, Weiss C, Casile A, Waszak F (2012) When sounds become actions: higher-order representation of newly learnt action sounds in the human motor system. J Cogn Neurosci 24(2):464–474Google Scholar
  46. Wallace RJ (1971) S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. J Exp Psychol 88:354–360CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Weigelt M, Rieger M, Mechsner F, Prinz W (2007) Target-related coupling in bimanual reaching movements. Psychol Res 71:438–447CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Wolfensteller U, Ruge H (2011) On the timescale of stimulus-based action-effect learning. Q J Exp Psychol 64:1273–1289CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology IIIJulius-Maximilians University of WürzburgWürzburgGermany

Personalised recommendations