Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 212, Issue 2, pp 305–314 | Cite as

Background sound modulates the performance of odor discrimination task

  • Han-Seok Seo
  • Volker Gudziol
  • Antje Hähner
  • Thomas Hummel
Research Article

Abstract

Even though we often perceive odors in the presence of various background sounds, surprisingly little is known about the effects of background sound on odor perception. This study aimed to investigate the question whether background sound can modulate performance in an odor discrimination task. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to perform the odor discrimination task while listening to either background noise (e.g., verbal or non-verbal noise) or no additional sound (i.e., silent condition). Participants’ performance in the odor discrimination task was significantly deteriorated in the presence of background noise compared with in the silent condition. Rather, the detrimental effect of verbal noise on the task performance was significantly higher than that of non-verbal noise. In Experiment 2, participants were asked to conduct the odor discrimination task while listening to either background music (Mozart’s sonata for two pianos in D major, K448) or no additional sound (silent condition). Background music relative to silent condition did not significantly alter the task performance. In conclusion, our findings provide new empirical evidence that background sound modulates the performance in an odor discrimination task.

Keywords

Auditory–olfactory integration Background noise Background music Odor discrimination task Task performance 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique to T.H. (European associated laboratory; EAL 549, CNRS-TUD).

References

  1. Belkin K, Martin R, Kemp SE, Gilbert AN (1997) Auditory pitch as a perceptual analogue to odor quality. Psychol Sci 8:340–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B, Slepcevic V (2003) Noise and mental performance: personality attributes and noise sensitivity. Noise Health 6:77–89PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bensafi M, Rouby C, Farget V, Bertrand B, Vigouroux M, Holley A (2003) Perceptual, affective, and cognitive judgments of odors: pleasantness and handedness effects. Brain Cogn 51:270–275PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bock O (2010) Sensorimotor adaptation is influenced by background music. Exp Brain Res 203:737–741PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boesveldt S, Verbaan D, Knol DL, Visser M, van Rooden SM, van Hilten JJ, Berendse HW (2008) A comparative study of odor identification and odor discrimination deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 23:1984–1990PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boman E, Enmarker I, Hygge S (2005) Strength of noise effects on memory as a function of noise source and age. Noise Health 7:11–26PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brosch M, Selezneva E, Schelch H (2005) Nonauditory events of a behavioral procedure activate auditory cortex of highly trained monkeys. J Neurosci 25:6797–6806PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Budinger E, Heil P, Hess A, Scheich H (2006) Multisensory processing via early cortical stages: connections of the primary auditory cortical filed with other sensory systems. Neuroscience 143:1065–1083PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cassidy G, MacDonald RAR (2007) The effect of background music and background noise on the task performance of introverts and extraverts. Psychol Music 35:517–537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chabris CF (1999) Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’? Nature 400:826–827PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chen D, Dalton P (2005) The effect of emotion and personality on olfactory perception. Chem Senses 30:345–351PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Crawford HJ, Strapp CM (1994) Effects of vocal and instrumental music on visuospatial and verbal performance as moderated by studying preference and personality. Person Individ Diff 16:237–245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Crisinel AS, Spence C (2009) Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neurosci Lett 464:39–42PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dolan RJ (2002) Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science 298:1191–1194PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Doyle PJ, Anderson DW, Pijl S (1984) The tuning fork: an essential instrument in otologic practice. J Otolaryngol 13:83–86PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Furnham A, Allass K (1999) The influence of musical distraction of varying complexity on the cognitive performance of extroverts and introverts. Eur J Pers 13:27–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Furnham A, Bradley A (1997) Music while you work: the differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extroverts. Appl Cogn Psychol 11:445–455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Furnham A, Strbac L (2002) Music is as distracting as noise: the differential distraction of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Ergonomics 45:203–217PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gottfried JA (2006) Smell: central nervous processing. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 63:44–69PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hairston WD, Hodges DA, Casanova R, Hayasaka S, Kraft R, Maldjian JA, Burdette JH (2008) Closing the mind’s eye: deactivation of visual cortex related to auditory task difficulty. Neuroreport 19:151–154PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hedner M, Larsson M, Arnold N, Zucco GM, Hummel T (2010) Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32:1062–1067PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ho C, Mason O, Spence C (2007) An investigation into the temporal dimension of the Mozart effect: evidence from the attentional blink task. Acta Psychol 125:117–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hodge B, Thompson JF (1990) Noise pollution in the operating theatre. Lancet 335:891–894PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G (1997) ‘Sniffin’ sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 22:39–52PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hummel T, Konnerth OG, Rosenheim K, Kobal G (2001) Screening of olfactory function with a four-minute odor identification test: reliability, normative data, and investigations in patients with olfactory loss. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 110:976–981PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hygge S (2003) Classroom experiments on the effects of different noise sources and sound levels on long-term recall and recognition in children. Appl Cogn Psychol 17:895–914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jäncke L, Sandmann P (2010) Music listening while you learn: no influence of background music on verbal learning. Behav Brain Funct 6:3PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Larsson M, Nilsson LG, Olofsson JK, Nordin S (2004) Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 29:547–554PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. LeCompte DC, Neely CB, Wilson JR (1997) Irrelevant speech and irrelevant tones: the relative importance of speech to the irrelevant speech effect. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:472–483PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mozolic J, Joyner D, Hugenschmidt CE, Peiffer AM, Kraft RA, Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ (2008) Cross-modal deactivations during modality-specific selective attention. BMC Neurol 8:35PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nantais KM, Schellenberg EG (1999) The Mozart effect: an artifact of preference. Psychol Sci 10:370–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Oldham GR, Cummings A, Mischel LJ, Schmidtke JM, Zhou J (1995) Listen while you work? Quasi-experimental relations between personal-stereo headset use and employee work responses. J Appl Psychol 80:547–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pietschnig J, Voracek M, Formann AK (2010) Mozart effect-Shmozart effect: a meta-analysis. Intelligence 38:314–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Plailly J, Tillmann B, Royet J-P (2007) The feeling of familiarity of music and odors: the same neural signature? Cereb Cortex 17:2650–2658PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rauscher FH, Shaw GL, Ky KN (1993) Music and spatial task performance. Nature 365:611PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 86:216–244PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Royet J-P, Plailly J, Delon-Martin C, Kareken DA, Segebarth C (2003) fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. Neuroimage 20:713–728PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Seo H-S, Hummel T (2011) Auditory-olfactory integration: congruent or pleasant sounds amplify odor pleasantness. Chem Senses 36:301–309PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Seo H-S, Jeon KJ, Hummel T, Min BC (2009) Influences of olfactory impairment on depression, cognitive performane, and quality of life in Korean elderly. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 266:1739–1745PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Smith WA (1961) Effects of industrial music in a work situation requiring complex mental activity. Psychol Rep 8:159–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spangenberg ER, Grohmann B, Sprott DE (2005) It’s a beginning to smell (and sound) a lot like Christmas: the interactive effects of ambient scent and music in a retail setting. J Bus Res 58:1583–1589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Steele KM, Bass KE, Crook MD (1999) The mystery of the Mozart effect: failure to replicate. Psychol Sci 10:366–369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Thompson WF, Schellenberg EG, Husain G (2001) Arousal, mood, and the Mozart effect. Psychol Sci 12:248–251PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Trimmel M, Poelzl G (2006) Impact of backgrond noise on reaction time and brain DC potential changes of VDT-based spatial attention. Ergonomics 49:202–208PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Turner ML, Fernandez JE, Nelson K (1996) The effect of music amplitude on the reaction to unexpected visual events. J Gen Psychol 123:51–62PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Van Gerven PW, Meijer WA, Vermeeren A, Vuurman EF, Jolles J (2007) The irrelevant speech effect and the level of interference in aging. Exp Aging Res 33:323–339PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Vickers ZM, Bourne MC (1976) A psychoacoustical theory of crispness. J Food Sci 41:1158–1164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Werner-Reiss U, Kelly KA, Trause AS, Underhill AM, Groh JM (2003) Eye position affects activity in primary auditory cortex of primates. Curr Biol 13:554–562PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wesson DW, Wilson DA (2010) Smelling sounds: olfactory-auditory sensory convergence in the olfactory tubercle. J Neurosci 30:3013–3021PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wijayanto T, Tochihara Y, Wijaya AR, Hermawati S (2009) Combined factors effect of menstrual cycle and background noise on visual inspection task performance: a simulation-based task. J Physiol Anthropol 28:253–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Woods AT, Poliakoff E, Lloyd DM, Kuenzel J, Hodson R, Gonda H, Batchelor J, Dijksterhuis GB, Thomas A (2011) Effect of background noise on food perception. Food Qual Pref 22:42–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Zampini M, Spence C (2004) The role of auditory cues in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of potato chips. J Sens Stud 19:347–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Han-Seok Seo
    • 1
  • Volker Gudziol
    • 1
  • Antje Hähner
    • 1
  • Thomas Hummel
    • 1
  1. 1.Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of OtorhinolaryngologyUniversity of Dresden Medical SchoolDresdenGermany

Personalised recommendations