The role of group membership on the modulation of joint action

  • Cristina Iani
  • Filomena Anelli
  • Roberto Nicoletti
  • Luciano Arcuri
  • Sandro Rubichi
Research Article

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to assess whether the emergence of shared representations, as indexed by the joint Simon effect, is modulated by perceived group membership. In both experiments, participants were required to perform a Simon task along another person who was perceived as belonging either to the same group or to a different group. In Experiment 1, ingroup–outgroup discrimination was obtained by dividing participants into two groups based on a superficial criterion; in Experiment 2, it was obtained by manipulating the interdependence experienced by the two acting individuals. The mere social categorization of co-acting participants into groups did not modulate the joint Simon effect which was observed even when participants believed to perform the task along with an individual belonging to a different social group (Experiment 1). On the contrary, the effect was modulated by perceived interdependence, with a null effect when participants experienced negative interdependence (Experiment 2). These results suggest that when acting in a social context, by default, individuals may perceive positive interdependence with co-acting individuals, even when cooperation is not explicitly requested.

Keywords

Simon effect Joint action Social categorization Ingroup and outgroup categorization Interdependence 

References

  1. Allen VL, Wilder DA (1975) Categorization, belief similarity, and intergroup discrimination. J Pers Soc Psychol 32:971–977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allen VL, Wilder DA (1979) Group categorization and attribution of belief similarity. Small Group Behav 10:73–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aron A, Aron EN, Tudor M, Nelson G (1991) Close relationships as including the other in the self. J Pers Soc Psychol 60:241–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Atmaca S, Sebanz N, Prinz W, Knoblich G (2008) Action co-representation: the joint SNARC effect. Soc Neurosci 3:410–420PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Billig M, Tajfel H (1973) Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behavior. Eur J Soc Psychol 3:27–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cadinu MR, Rothbart M (1996) Self-anchoring and differentiation processes in the minimal group setting. J Pers Soc Psychol 70:661–677PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. De Jong R, Liang C-C, Lauber E (1994) Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. J Exp Psychol Hum 20:731–750CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Deutsch M (1949) A theory of cooperation and competition. Hum Relat 2:129–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Deutsch M (1962) Cooperation and trust: some theoretical notes. In: Jones MR (ed) Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp 275–320Google Scholar
  10. Doise W, Deschamps JC, Meyer G (1978) The accentuation of intra-category similarities. In: Tajfel H (ed) Differentiation between social groups. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  11. Hommel B, Colzato LS, van den Wildenberg WPM (2009) How social are task representations? Psychol Sci 20:794–798PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kelley JJ, Thiabut JW (1969) Group problem solving. In: Lindzey G, Aronson E (eds) Handbook of social psychology vol 4, 2nd edn. Addison-Wesley, Reading, pp 1–101Google Scholar
  13. Kuhbandner C, Pekrum R, Maier MA (2010) The role of positive and negative affect in the “mirroring” of other person’s actions. Cognition Emotion 24:1182–1190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lewin K (1948) Resolving social conflicts. Selected papers on group dynamics. Harper & Row, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Milanese N, Iani C, Rubichi S (2010) Shared learning shapes human performance: transfer effects in task sharing. Cognition 116:15–22PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Poortvliet PM, Darnon C (2010) Toward a more social understanding of achievement goals: the interpersonal effects of mastery and performance goals. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:324–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Proctor RW, Vu K-PL (2006) Stimulus–response compatibility principle: data, theory, and application. Taylor & Francis, Boca RatonGoogle Scholar
  18. Rabbie JM, Horwitz M (1969) Arousal of ingroup–outgroup bias by a chance win or loss. J Pers Soc Psychol 13:269–277PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Rubichi S, Pellicano A (2004) Does the Simon effect affect movement execution? Eur J Cogn Psychol 16:825–840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Rubichi S, Nicoletti R, Umiltà C, Zorzi M (2000) Response strategies and the Simon effect. Psychol Res 63:129–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ruys KI, Aarts H (2010) When competition merges people’s behavior: interdependency activates shared action representations. J Exp Soc Psychol 46:1130–1133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88:11–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2005) How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. J Exp Psychol Hum 31:1234–1246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006a) Joint actions: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10:70–76PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W, Wascher E (2006b) Twin peaks: an ERP study of action planning and control in co-acting individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 18:859–870PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Sebanz N, Rebbechi D, Knoblich G, Prinz W, Frith CD (2007) Is it really my turn? An event-related fMRI study of task sharing. Soc Neurosci 2:81–95PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Simon JR, Rudell AP (1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. J Appl Psychol 51:300–304PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Smith ER, Henry S (1996) An ingroup becomes part of the self: response time evidence. Pers Soc Psychol B 22:635–642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Tajfel H (1970) Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Sci Am 223:96–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Tajfel H, Billig M (1974) Familiarity and categorization in intergroup behavior. J Exp Soc Psychol 10:159–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Tajfel H, Billig M, Bundy RP, Flament C (1971) Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur J Soc Psychol 2:149–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28:675–735PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Tsai C-C, Kuo WJ, Jing JT, Hung DL, Tzeng OJL (2006) A common coding framework in self-other interaction: evidence from joint action task. Exp Brain Res 175:353–362PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tsai CC, Kuo WJ, Hung DL, Tzeng OJL (2008) Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. J Cogn Neurosci 20:2015–2024PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Welsh TN, Higgins L, Ray M, Weeks DJ (2007) Seeing vs. believing: is believing sufficient to activate the process of response co-representation? Hum Mov Sci 26:853–866PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cristina Iani
    • 1
  • Filomena Anelli
    • 2
  • Roberto Nicoletti
    • 2
  • Luciano Arcuri
    • 3
  • Sandro Rubichi
    • 1
  1. 1.Dipartimento di Comunicazione e EconomiaUniversità di Modena e Reggio EmiliaReggio EmiliaItaly
  2. 2.Dipartimento di Discipline della ComunicazioneUniversità di BolognaBolognaItaly
  3. 3.Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della SocializzazioneUniversità di PadovaPadovaItaly

Personalised recommendations