Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 209, Issue 4, pp 481–493 | Cite as

The role of the occipital face area in the cortical face perception network

Review

Abstract

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have identified spatially distinct face-selective regions in human cortex. These regions have been linked together to form the components of a cortical network specialized for face perception but the cognitive operations performed in each region are not well understood. In this paper, we review the evidence concerning one of these face-selective regions, the occipital face area (OFA), to better understand what cognitive operations it performs in the face perception network. Neuropsychological evidence and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies demonstrate the OFA is necessary for accurate face perception. fMRI and TMS studies investigating the functional role of the OFA suggest that it preferentially represents the parts of a face, including the eyes, nose, and mouth and that it does so at an early stage of visual perception. These studies are consistent with the hypothesis that the OFA is the first stage in a hierarchical face perception network in which the OFA represents facial components prior to subsequent processing of increasingly complex facial features in higher face-selective cortical regions.

Keywords

Face perception Occipital face area (OFA) Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank Galit Yovel and Danny Dilks for their typically perceptive comments and Marius Peelen and Boaz Sadeh for supplying figures. This work was supported by BBSRC grant BB/F022875/1 to BD and VW.

References

  1. Allison T, Puce A, Spencer DD, McCarthy G (1999) Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: potentials generated in occipito-temporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cereb Cortex 9:415–430PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000) Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4:267–278PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barbeau EJ, Taylor MJ, Regis J, Marquis P, Chauvel P, Liegeois-Chauvel C (2008) Spatio temporal dynamics of face recognition. Cereb Cortex 18:997–1009PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barton JJS (2008) Structure and function in acquired prosospagnosia: lessons from a series of ten patients with brain damage. J Neuropsychol 2:197–225PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barton JJS, Press DZ, Keenan JP, O’Connor M (2002) Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial configuration in prosopagnosia. Neurology 58:71–78PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G (1996) Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 8:551–565PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bouvier SE, Engel SA (2006) Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia. Cereb Cortex 16:183–191PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bruce V, Young A (1986) Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol 77:305–327PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Busigny T, Graf M, Mayer E, Rossion B (2010) Acquired prosopagnosia as a face-specific disorder: ruling out the general visual similarity account. Neuropsychologia 48:2051–2067PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Calder AJ, Young AW (2005) Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:641–651PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark VP, Keil K, Maisog JM, Courtney S, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1996) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human visual cortex during face matching: a comparison with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 4:1–15PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Downing P, Jiang Y, Shuman M, Kanwisher N (2001) A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science 293:2470–2473PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dricot L, Sorger B, Schiltz C, Goebel R, Rossion B (2008) The roles of “face” and “non-face” areas during individual face perception: evidence by fMRI adaptation in a brain-damaged prosopagnosic patient. NeuroImage 40:318–332Google Scholar
  14. Eimer M (1998) Does the face-specific N170 component reflect the activity of a specialized eye processor? Neuroreport 9:2945–2948PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Evans J, Heggs A, Antoun N, Hodges J (1995) Progressive prosopagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy: a new syndrome? Brain 118:1–13Google Scholar
  16. Fairhall SL, Ishai A (2007) Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical network for face perception. Cereb Cortex 17:2400–2406PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Farah MJ (2004) Visual agnosia: disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  18. Fox CJ, Moon S-Y, Iaria G, Barton JJS (2009) The correlates of subjective perception of identity and expression in the face network: an fMRI adaptation study. Neuroimage 44:569–580PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW (2000) The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci 12:495–504PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Grill-Spector K, Malach R (2004) The human visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:649–677PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1998) A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 6:316–328PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1999) Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron 24:187–203PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N (2004) The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nat Neurosci 7:555–562PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A (2006) Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus specific effects. Trends Cogn Sci 10:14–23PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Harris A, Aguirre GK (2008) The representation of parts and wholes in face-selective cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 20:863–878PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Grady CL (1994) The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a pet-rcbf study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 14:6336–6353PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG, Clark VP, Schouten JL, Hoffman EA, Martin A (1999) The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22:189–199PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI (2000) The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci 4:223–233PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Haxby JV, Gobbini MI, Furey ML, Ishai A, Schouten JL, Pietrini P (2001) Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293:2425–2430PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hemond CC, Kanwisher N, Op de Beeck HP (2007) A preference for contralateral stimuli in human object- and face-selective cortex. PLoS ONE 2(6):e574PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Henson RN, Goshen-Gottstein Y, Ganel T, Otten LJ, Quayle A, Rugg MD (2003) Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition and priming. Cereb Cortex 13:793–805PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Herrmann MJ, Ehlis A-C, Ellgring H, Fallgatter AJ (2005) Early stages (P100) of face perception in humans as measured with event-related potentials (ERPs). J Neural Transm 112:1073–1081PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hoffman EA, Haxby JV (2000) Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat Neurosci 3:80–84PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Horovitz SG, Rossion B, Skudlarski P, Gore JC (2004) Parametric design and correlational analyses help integrating fMRI and electrophysiological data during face processing. Neuroimage 22:1587–1595PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ishai A (2008) Let’s face it: it’s a cortical network. Neuroimage 40(2):415–419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ishai A, Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG (2002) Visual imagery of famous faces: effects of memory and attention revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage 17:1729–1741PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Itier RJ, Taylor MJ (2004) N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb Cortex 14:132–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Itier RJ, Herdman AT, George N, Cheyne D, Taylor MJ (2006) Inversion and contrast-reversal effects on face processing assessed by MEG. Brain Res 1115:108–120PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kanwisher N, Yovel G (2006) The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 361:2109–2128PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17:4302–4311PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kourtzi Z, Tolias AS, Altmann CF, Augath M, Logothetis NK (2003) Integration of local features into global shapes: monkey and human fMRI studies. Neuron 37:333–346PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kovács G, Cziraki C, Vidnyánszky Z, Schweinberger SR, Greenlee MW (2008) Position-specific and position-invariant face aftereffects reflect the adaptation of different cortical areas. Neuroimage 43:156–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kriegeskorte N, Formisano E, Sorger B, Goebel R (2007) Individual faces elicit distinct response patterns in human anterior temporal cortex. PNAS 104:20600–20605PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Large ME, Cavina-Pratesi C, Vilis T, Culham JC (2008) The neural correlates of change detection in the face perception network. Neuropsychologia 46(8):2169–2176PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lerner Y, Hendler T, Ben-Nashat D, Harel M, Malach R (2001) A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 11:287–297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Liu J, Harris A, Kanwisher N (2002) Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nat Neurosci 5:910–916PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Liu J, Harris A, Kanwisher N (2010) Perception of face parts and face configurations: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 22:203–211PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Malach R, Reppas J, Benson R, Kwong K, Jiang H, Kennedy W, Ledden P, Brady T, Rosen B, Tootell R (1995) Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. PNAS 92:8135–8139PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Martinez A, Di Russo F, Anllo-Vento L, Sereno MI, Buxton RB, Hillyard SA (2001) Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas. Vis Res 41:1437–1457PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. McCarthy G, Puce A, Gore J, Allison T (1997) Face-specific processing in the fusiform gyrus. J Cogn Neurosci 9:605–610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. McCarthy G, Puce A, Belger A, Allison T (1999) Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. II: response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex. Cereb Cortex 9:431–444PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Milner AD, Perrett DI, Johnston RS, Benson PJ, Jordan TR, Heeley DW, Bettucci D, Mortara F, Mutani R, Terassi E, Davidson DL (1991) Perception and action in “visual form agnosia”. Brain 114:405–428PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Moeller S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY (2008) Patches with links: a unified system for processing faces in the macaque temporal lobe. Science 320:1355–1359PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nestor A, Vettel JM, Tarr MJ (2008) Task-specific codes for face recognition: how they shape the neural representation of features for detection and individuation. Plos ONE 3:e3978PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Nichols DF, Betts LR, Wilson HR (2010) Decoding of faces and face components in face-sensitive human visual cortex. Front Psychol 1:28. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00028 Google Scholar
  56. Peelen MV, Lucas N, Mayer E, Vuilleumier P (2009) Emotional attention in acquired prosopagnosia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 4:268–277PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Pitcher D, Walsh V, Yovel G, Duchaine B (2007) TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing. Curr Biol 17(18):1568–1573PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Pitcher D, Garrido L, Walsh V, Duchaine B (2008) TMS disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions. J Neurosci 28(36):8929–8933PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pitcher D, Charles L, Devlin JT, Walsh V, Duchaine B (2009) Triple dissociation of faces, bodies, and objects in extrastriate cortex. Curr Biol 19(4):319–324PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Puce A, Allison T, Asgari M, Gore JC, McCarthy G (1996) Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 16:5205–5215PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Puce A, Allison T, McCarthy G (1999) Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. III: effects of top-down processing on face-specific potentials. Cereb Cortex 9:445–458PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Ramon M, Rossion B (2010) Impaired processing of relative distances between features and of the eye region in acquired prosopagnosia—two sides of the same holistic coin? Cortex 46:374–389PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Ramon M, Busigny T, Rossion B (2010a) Impaired holistic processing of unfamiliar individual faces in acquired prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 48:933–944PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Ramon M, Dricot L, Rossion B (2010b) Personally familiar faces are perceived categorically in face-selective regions other than the FFA. Eur J Neurosci 32:1587–1598PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rhodes G, Michie PT, Hughes ME, Byatt G (2009) FFA and OFA show sensitivity to spatial relations in faces. Eur J Neurosci 30:721–733PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Robertson I, Murre JMJ (1999) Rehabilitation of brain damage: brain plasticity and principles of guided recovery. Psychol Bull 125:544–575PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rosazza C, Cai Q, Minati l, Paulignan Y, Nazir T (2009) Early involvement of dorsal and ventral pathways in visual word recognition: an ERP study. Brain Res 1272:32–44Google Scholar
  68. Rossion B (2008) Constraining the cortical face network by neuroimaging studies of acquired prosopagnosia. Neuroimage 40:423–426PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rossion B, Jacques C (2008) Does physical interstimulus variance account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the human brain? Ten lessons on the N170. Neuroimage 39:1959–1979PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rossion B, de Gelder B, Dricot L, Zoontjes R, De Volder A, Bodart J-M, Crommelinck M (2000) Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J Cogn Neurosci 12:793–802PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rossion B, Caldara R, Seghier M, Schuller AM, Lazeyras F, Mayer E (2003) A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain 126:2381–2395PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rotshtein P, Henson RN, Treves A, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2005) Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brain. Nat Neurosci 8:107–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Rotshtein P, Geng JJ, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2007) Role of features and second-order spatial relations in face discrimination, face recognition, and individual face skills: behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging data. J Cogn Neurosci 19:1435–1452PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Sadeh B, Podlipsky I, Zadanov A, Yovel G (2010) Face-selective fMRI and event-related potential responses are highly correlated: evidence from simultaneous ERP-fMRI investigation. Hum Brain Mapp 31:1490–1501PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Schiltz C, Rossion B (2006) Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex. Neuroimage 32:1385–1394PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Schiltz C, Dricot L, Goebel R, Rossion B (2010) Holistic perception of individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus as evidenced by the composite face illusion. J Vis 10:1–16PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Schwarzlose RF, Swisher JD, Dang S, Kanwisher N (2008) The distribution of category and location information across object-selective regions in human visual cortex. PNAS 105:4447–4452PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B (1992) Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. Brain 115:15–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Silvanto J, Schwarzkopf DS, Gilaie-Dotan S, Rees G (2010) Differing causal roles for lateral occipital cortex and occipital face area in invariant shape recognition. Eur J Neurosci 32:165–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Slotnick SD (2004) Source localization of ERP generators. In: Handy TC (ed) Event-related potentials: a methods handbook. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 149–166Google Scholar
  81. Spiridon M, Kanwisher N (2002) How distributed is visual category information in human occipital-temporal cortex? An fMRI study. Neuron 35:1157–1165PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Steeves JKE, Dricot L, Goltz HC, Sorger B, Peters J, Milner AD, Goodale MA, Goebel R, Rossion B (2009) Abnormal face identity coding in the middle fusiform gyrus of two brain-damaged prosopagnosic patients. Neuropsychologia 47:2584–2592PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Thierry G, Martin CD, Downing PE, Pegna AJ (2007) Controlling for interstimulus perceptual variance abolishes N170 face selectivity. Nat Neurosci 10:505–511PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Tootell RBH, Livingstone M (2006) A cortical region consisting entirely of face cells. Science 311:670–674PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Tsao DY, Moeller S, Freiwald WA (2008) Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. PNAS 105:19514–19519PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Ullman S, Vidal-Naquet M, Sali E (2002) Visual features of intermediate complexity and their use in classification. Nat Neurosci 5:682–687PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Watson JD, Myers R, Frackowiak RS, Hajnal JV, Woods RP, Mazziotta J, Zeki S (1993) Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 3:79–94PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K (2010) Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex. NeuroImage 52:1559–1573PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Young AW, Hay DC, McWeeny KH, Ellis AW, Barry C (1985) Familiarity decisions for faces presented to the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Brain Cogn 4:439–450PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Young AW, Hellawell D, Hay DC (1987) Configurational information in face perception. Perception 16:747–759PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Yovel G, Kanwisher N (2004) Face perception: domain specific, not process specific. Neuron 44:889–898Google Scholar
  92. Yovel G, Kanwisher N (2005) The neural basis of the behavioral face-inversion effect. Curr Biol 15:2256–2262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Pitcher
    • 1
  • Vincent Walsh
    • 1
  • Bradley Duchaine
    • 2
  1. 1.Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.Department of Psychology and Brain SciencesDartmouth CollegeHanoverUSA

Personalised recommendations