Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 209, Issue 3, pp 385–393

Basal ganglia-dependent processes in recalling learned visual-motor adaptations

Research Article
  • 408 Downloads

Abstract

Humans learn and remember motor skills to permit adaptation to a changing environment. During adaptation, the brain develops new sensory–motor relationships that become stored in an internal model (IM) that may be retained for extended periods. How the brain learns new IMs and transforms them into long-term memory remains incompletely understood since prior work has mostly focused on the learning process. A current model suggests that basal ganglia, cerebellum, and their neocortical targets actively participate in forming new IMs but that a cerebellar cortical network would mediate automatization. However, a recent study (Marinelli et al. 2009) reported that patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD), who have basal ganglia dysfunction, had similar adaptation rates as controls but demonstrated no savings at recall tests (24 and 48 h). Here, we assessed whether a longer training session, a feature known to increase long-term retention of IM in healthy individuals, could allow PD patients to demonstrate savings. We recruited PD patients and age-matched healthy adults and used a visual-motor adaptation paradigm similar to the study by Marinelli et al. (2009), doubling the number of training trials and assessed recall after a short and a 24-h delay. We hypothesized that a longer training session would allow PD patients to develop an enhanced representation of the IM as demonstrated by savings at the recall tests. Our results showed that PD patients had similar adaptation rates as controls but did not demonstrate savings at both recall tests. We interpret these results as evidence that fronto-striatal networks have involvement in the early to late phase of motor memory formation, but not during initial learning.

Keywords

Visual-motor learning Basal ganglia Parkinson’s disease Long-term memory Recall Internal models 

References

  1. Agostino R, Curra A, Soldati G, Dinapoli L, Chiacchiari L, Modugno N, Pierelli F, Berardelli A (2004) Prolonged practice is of scarce benefit in improving motor performance in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 19:1285–1293PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bédard P, Sanes JN (2009) On a basal ganglia role in learning and rehearsing visual-motor associations. Neuroimage 47:1701–1710PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Behrman AL, Cauraugh JH, Light KE (2000) Practice as an intervention to improve speeded motor performance and motor learning in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 174:127–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Hallett M (2001) Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 124:2131–2146PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bock O, Schneider S, Bloomberg J (2001) Conditions for interference versus facilitation during sequential sensorimotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 138:359–365PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boecker H, Ceballos-Baumann A, Bartenstein P, Weindl A, Siebner HR, Fassbender T, Munz F, Schwaiger M, Conrad B (1999) Sensory processing in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease: investigations with 3D H(2)(15)O-PET. Brain 122:1651–1665PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzi E (1996) Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382:252–255PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Caithness G, Osu R, Bays P, Chase H, Klassen J, Kawato M, Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2004) Failure to consolidate the consolidation theory of learning for sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurosci 24:8662–8671PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Contreras-Vidal JL, Buch ER (2003) Effects of Parkinson’s disease on visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 150:25–32PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Demirci M, Grill S, McShane L, Hallett M (1997) A mismatch between kinesthetic and visual perception in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 41:781–788PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Doyon J, Benali H (2005) Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:161–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Doyon J, Laforce RJ, Bouchard G, Gaudreau D, Roy J, Poirier M, Bedard PJ, Bedard F, Bouchard JP (1998) Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements. Neuropsychologia 36:625–641PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Doyon J, Song AW, Karni A, Lalonde F, Adams MM, Ungerleider LG (2002) Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:1017–1022PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Doyon J, Penhune V, Ungerleider LG (2003) Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia 41:252–262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Doyon J, Bellec P, Amsel R, Penhune V, Monchi O, Carrier J, Lehericy S, Benali H (2009) Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behav Brain Res 199:61–75PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Floyer-Lea A, Matthews PM (2004) Changing brain networks for visuomotor control with increased movement automaticity. J Neurophysiol 92:2405–2412PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Floyer-Lea A, Matthews PM (2005) Distinguishable brain activation networks for short- and long-term motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol 94:512–518PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Graybiel AM (2005) The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:638–644PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Grol MJ, de Lange FP, Verstraten FA, Passingham RE, Toni I (2006) Cerebral changes during performance of overlearned arbitrary visuomotor associations. J Neurosci 26:117–125PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Joiner WM, Smith MA (2008) Long-term retention explained by a model of short-term learning in the adaptive control of reaching. J Neurophysiol 100:2948–2955PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Klassen J, Tong C, Flanagan JR (2005) Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations. Exp Brain Res 164:250–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Klockgether T, Borutta M, Rapp H, Spieker S, Dichgans J (1995) A defect of kinesthesia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 10:460–465PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Mentis M, Barnes A, Veytsman M, Eidelberg D, Ghez C (2004) Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. J Neurophysiol 91:924–933PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Krakauer JW, Ghez C, Ghilardi MF (2005) Adaptation to visuomotor transformations: consolidation, interference, and forgetting. J Neurosci 25:473–478PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Krebs HI, Hogan N, Hening W, Adamovich SV, Poizner H (2001) Procedural motor learning in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res 141:425–437PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Krueger WCF (1929) The effect of overlearning on retention. J Exp Psychol 12:71–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Luh CW (1922) The conditions of retention. Psychol Monogr 31:1–87Google Scholar
  30. Marinelli L, Crupi D, Di Rocco A, Bove M, Eidelberg D, Abbruzzese G, Ghilardi MF (2009) Learning and consolidation of visuo-motor adaptation in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 15:6–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2006) An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 26:3642–3645PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Messier J, Adamovich S, Jack D, Hening W, Sage J, Poizner H (2007) Visuomotor learning in immersive 3D virtual reality in Parkinson’s disease and in aging. Exp Brain Res 179:457–474PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mochizuki-Kawai H, Kawamura M, Hasegawa Y, Mochizuki S, Oeda R, Yamanaka K, Tagaya H (2004) Deficits in long-term retention of learned motor skills in patients with cortical or subcortical degeneration. Neuropsychologia 42:1858–1863PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B (2007) Motor procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 130:2887–2897PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Nezafat R, Shadmehr R, Holcomb HH (2001) Long-term adaptation to dynamics of reaching movements: a PET study. Exp Brain Res 140:66–76PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Paquet F, Bedard MA, Levesque M, Tremblay PL, Lemay M, Blanchet PJ, Scherzer P, Chouinard S, Filion J (2008) Sensorimotor adaptation in Parkinson’s disease: evidence for a dopamine dependent remapping disturbance. Exp Brain Res 185:227–236PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Penhune VB, Doyon J (2002) Dynamic cortical and subcortical networks in learning and delayed recall of timed motor sequences. J Neurosci 22:1397–1406PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. R Development Core Team (2009) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, AustriaGoogle Scholar
  41. Rand MK, Stelmach GE, Bloedel JR (2000) Movement accuracy constraints in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia 38:203–212PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schultz W (2006) Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annu Rev Psychol 57:87–115PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Seidler RD, Noll DC, Chintalapati P (2006) Bilateral basal ganglia activation associated with sensorimotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 175:544–555PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Seidler RD, Tuite P, Ashe J (2007) Selective impairments in implicit learning in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res 1137:104–110PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Shadmehr R, Holcomb HH (1997) Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science 277:821–825PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Shadmehr R, Holcomb HH (1999) Inhibitory control of competing motor memories. Exp Brain Res 126:235–251PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Shadmehr R, Wise SP (2005) The computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing: a foundation for motor learning. MIT press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  48. Smiley-Oyen AL, Lowry KA, Emerson QR (2006) Learning and retention of movement sequences in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 21:1078–1087PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith MA, Shadmehr R (2005) Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington’s disease but not cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol 93:2809–2821PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Stern Y, Mayeux R, Hermann A, Rosen J (1988) Prism adaptation in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:1584–1587PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wang XP, Sun BM, Ding HL (2009) Changes of procedural learning in Chinese patients with non-demented Parkinson disease. Neurosci Lett 449:161–163PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Weiner MJ, Hallett M, Funkenstein HH (1983) Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system. Neurology 33:766–772PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Yin PB, Kitazawa S (2001) Long-lasting aftereffects of prism adaptation in the monkey. Exp Brain Res 141:250–253PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Neuroscience, Alpert Medical School of Brown UniversityProvidenceUSA

Personalised recommendations