Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 209, Issue 3, pp 375–384 | Cite as

Is inefficient multisensory processing associated with falls in older people?

  • Annalisa Setti
  • Kate E. Burke
  • Rose Anne Kenny
  • Fiona N. Newell
Research Article

Abstract

Although falling is a significant problem for older persons, little is understood about its underlying causes. Spatial cognition and balance maintenance rely on the efficient integration of information across the main senses. We investigated general multisensory efficiency in older persons with a history of falls compared to age- and sensory acuity-matched controls and younger adults using a sound-induced flash illusion. Older fallers were as susceptible to the illusion as age-matched, non-fallers or younger adults at a short delay of 70 ms between the auditory and visual stimuli. Both older adult groups were more susceptible to the illusion at longer SOAs than younger adults. However, with increasing delays between the visual and auditory stimuli, older fallers did not show a decline in the frequency at which the illusion was experienced even with delays of up to 270 ms. We argue that this relatively higher susceptibility to the illusion reflects inefficient audio–visual processing in the central nervous system and has important implications for the diagnosis and rehabilitation of falling in older persons.

Keywords

Ageing Falls Older persons Cross-modal Perception Multisensory Integration Temporal window Balance 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was completed as part of a wider programme of research within the TRIL Centre, (Technology Research for Independent Living). The TRIL Centre is a multidisciplinary research centre, bringing together researchers from UCD, TCD, NUIG & Intel, funded by Intel, IDA Ireland and GE Healthcare. http://www.trilcentre.org

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to report.

References

  1. Abdelhafiz AH, Austin CA (2003) Visual factors should be assessed in older people presenting with falls or hip fracture. Age Ageing 32:26–30PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allison L, Jeka JJ (2004) Multisensory integration: resolving ambiguities for human postural control. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) The handbook of multisensory processes. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 785–797Google Scholar
  3. Allison LK, Kiemel T, Jeka JJ (2006) Multisensory reweighting of vision and touch is intact in healthy and fall-prone older adults. Exp Brain Res 75:342–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Andersen TS, Tiippana K, Sams M (2004) Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fusion illusions. Cogn Brain Res 21:301–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barnett-Cowan M, Harris LR (2009) Perceived timing of vestibular stimulation relative to touch, light and sound. Exp Brain Res 198:221–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barnett-Cowan M, Dyde RT, Thompson C, Harris LR (2010) Multisensory determinants of orientation perception: task-specific sex differences. Eur J Neurosci 31:1899–1907PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Dubost V, Allali G, Kressig RW, Bridenbaugh S, Berrut G, Assal F, Herrmann FR (2009) Stops walking when talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur J Neurol 16:786–795PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berg K, Wood-Dauphinee S, Williams JI, Maki B (1992) Measuring balance in the elderly: validation of an instrument. Can J Public Health July/August supplement S7–S11Google Scholar
  9. Blake AJ, Morgan K, Bendall MJ, Dallosso H, Ebrahim SB, Arie TH, Fentem PH, Bassey EJ (1988) Falls by elderly people at home: prevalence and associated factors. Age Ageing 17:365–372PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cadieux ML, Barnett-Cowan M, Shore DI (2010) Crossing the hands is more confusing for females than males. Exp Brain Res 204:431–446PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Calvert G, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) (2004) The handbook of multisensory processes. M.I.T. press, BostonGoogle Scholar
  12. Darlington CL, Smith PF (1998) Further evidence for gender differences in circularvection. J Vestib Res 8:151–153PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Davies AJ, Kenny RA (1996) Falls presenting to the accident and emergency department: types of presentation and risk factor profile. Age Ageing 25:362–366PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. De Sanctis P, Katz R, Wylie GR, Sehatpour P, Alexopoulos GS, Foxe JJ (2008) Enhanced and bilateralized visual sensory processing in the ventral stream may be a feature of normal aging. Neurobiol Aging 29:1576–1586PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Diederich A, Colonius H, Schomburg A (2008) Assessing age-related multisensory enhancement with the time-window-of-integration model. Neuropsychologia 46:2556–2562PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dingwell JD, Robb RT, Troy KL, Grabiner MD (2008) Effect of an attention demanding task on dynamic stability during treadmill walking. J NeuroEng Rehabil 5:12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Driver J, Spence C (2000) Multisensory perception: beyond modularity and convergence. Curr Biol 10:R731–R735PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH (2004) Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn Sci 8:162–169PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fitzgibbons PJ, Gordon-Salant S (1998) Auditory temporal order perception in younger and older adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res 41:1052–1060PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fozard JL, Gordon-Salant S (2001) Changes in vision and hearing with aging. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW (eds) Handbook of the psychology of aging. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 241–266Google Scholar
  21. Ghazanfar AA, Schroeder CE (2006) Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn Sci 10:278–285PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gordon-Salant S (2005) Hearing loss and aging: new research findings and clinical implications. J Rehabil Res Dev 42:9–24PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grady CL (2008) Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Ann NY Acad Sci 1124:127–144PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hasher L, Zacks RT (1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory, vol 22. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 193–225Google Scholar
  25. Hausdorff JM, Schweiger A, Herman T, Yogev-Seligmann G, Giladi N (2008) Dual-task decrements in gait: contributing factors among healthy older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 63:1335–1343PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Horak FB, Shupert CL, Mirka A (1989) Components of postural dyscontrol in elderly: a review. Neurobiol Aging 10:727–738Google Scholar
  27. Hugenschmidt CE, Mozolic J, Tan H, Kraft R, Laurienti P (2009) Age-related increase in cross-sensory noise in resting and steady-state cerebral perfusion. Brain Topogr 21:241–251PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Innes-Brown H, Crewther D (2009) The impact of spatial incongruence on an auditory-visual illusion. PLoS One 4:e6450PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ivy GO, MacLeod CM, Petit TL, Markus EJ (1992) A physiological framework for perceptual and cognitive changes in aging. In: Craik FIM, Salthouse TA (eds) The handbook of aging and cognition, vol 1. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 273–314Google Scholar
  30. Jeka JJ, Allison LK, Kiemel T (2010) The dynamics of visual reweighting in healthy and fall-prone older adults. J Mot Behav 42:197–208PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kennedy RS, Hettinger LJ, Harm DL, Ordy JM, Dunlap WP (1996) Psychophysical scaling of circular vection (CV) produced by optokinetic (OKN) motion: individual differences and effects of practice. J Vestib Res 6:331–341PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kulmala J, Viljanen A, Sipila S, Pajala S, Parssinen O, Kauppinen M, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, Rantanen T (2009) Poor vision accompanied with other sensory impairments as a predictor of falls in older women. Age Ageing 38:162–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lajoie Y, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M (1993) Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium. Exp Brain Res 97:139–144PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Laurienti PJ, Burdette JH, Maldjian JA, Wallace MT (2006) Enhanced multisensory integration in older adults. Neurobiol Aging 27:1155–1163PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lord SR (2006) Visual risk factors for falls in older people. Age Ageing 35:ii42–45PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lord SR, Menz HB (2000) Visual contributions to postural stability in older adults. Gerontology 46:306–310PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lord SR, Clark RD, Webster IW (1991) Physiological factors associated with falls in an elderly population. J Am Geriatr Soc 39:1194–1200PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Mishra J, Martinez A, Hillyard SA (2008) Cortical processes underlying sound-induced flash fusion. Brain Res 1242:102–115PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mozolic J, Long A, Morgan A, Rawley-Payne M, Laurienti PJ (2009) A cognitive training intervention improves modality-specific attention in a randomized controlled trial of healthy older adults. Neurobiol Aging. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.013
  40. O’Loughlin JL, Robitaille Y, Boivin JF, Suissa S (1993) Incidence of and risk factors for falls and injurious falls among the community-dwelling elderly. Am J Epidemiol 137:342–354PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Pajala S, Era P, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, Törmäkangas T, Rantanen T (2008) Force platform balance measures as predictors of indoor and outdoor falls in community-dwelling women aged 63–76 years. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 63:171–178PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Peiffer AM, Mozolic JL, Hugenschmidt CE, Laurienti PJ (2007) Age-related multisensory enhancement in a simple audiovisual detection task. Neuroreport 18:1077–1081PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Peterka RJ (2002) Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol 88:1097–1118PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Piirtola M, Era P (2006) Force platform measurements as predictors of falls among older people—a review. Gerontology 52:1–16PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S (1991) The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 39:142–148PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Poliakoff E, Ashworth S, Lowe C, Spence C (2006a) Vision and touch in ageing: crossmodal selective attention and visuotactile spatial interactions. Neuropsychologia 44:507–517PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Poliakoff E, Shore DI, Lowe C, Spence C (2006b) Visuotactile temporal order judgments in ageing. Neurosci Lett 396:207–211PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Powers AR III, Hillock AR, Wallace MT (2009) Perceptual training narrows the temporal window of multisensory binding. J Neurosci 29:12265–12274PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Romero-Ortuno R, Cogan L, Cunningham CU, Kenny RA (2010) Do older pedestrians have enough time to cross roads in Dublin? A critique of the traffic management guidelines based on clinical research findings. Age Ageing 39:80–86PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Schieber F (2006) Vision and aging. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW (eds) Handbook of psychology and aging, 6th edn. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 129–161Google Scholar
  51. Scialfa CT (2002) The role of sensory factors in cognitive aging research. Can J Exp Psychol 56:153–163Google Scholar
  52. Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S (2000) What you see is what you hear. Nature 408:788–788PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Shams L, Kamitani Y, Thompson S, Shimojo S (2001) Sound alters visual evoked potentials in humans. Neuroreport 12:3849–3852PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S (2002) A visual illusion induced by sound. Cogn Brain Res 14:147–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Stein BE, Meredith A (1993) The merging of the senses. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  56. Teasdale N, Simoneau M (2001) Attentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegration. Gait Posture 14:203–210PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tinetti ME, Williams CS (1998) The effect of falls and fall injuries on functioning in community-dwelling older persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 53:M112–M119PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Tinetti ME, Speechley M, Ginter SF (1988) Risk factors for falls among elderly persons living in the community. N Engl J Med 319:1701–1707PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tromp AM, Pluijm SMF, Smit JH, Deeg DJH, Bouter LM, Lips P (2001) Fall-risk screening test: a prospective study on predictors for falls in community-dwelling elderly. J Clin Epidemiol 54:837–844PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Ulbrich P, Churan J, Fink M, Wittmann M (2009) Perception of temporal order: the effects of age, sex, and cognitive factors. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 16:183–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Viaud-Delmon I, Ivanenko YP, Berthoz A, Jouvent R (1998) Sex, lies and virtual reality. Nat Neurosci 1:15–16PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Viljanen A, Kaprio J, Pyykkö I, Sorri M, Pajala S, Kauppinen M, Koskenvuo M, Rantanen T (2009) Hearing as a predictor of falls and postural balance in older female twins. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 64(A):312–317PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Watkins S, Shams L, Tanaka S, Haynes JD, Rees G (2006) Sound alters activity in human V1 in association with illusory visual perception. NeuroImage 31:1247–1256PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Willott JF (1991) Aging and the auditory system: anatomy, physiology, psychophysics. Singular, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  65. Woollacott MH (2000) Systems contributing to balance disorders in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55A:M424–M428Google Scholar
  66. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A (2002) Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16:1–14PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Annalisa Setti
    • 1
  • Kate E. Burke
    • 1
  • Rose Anne Kenny
    • 2
  • Fiona N. Newell
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Psychology, Institute of Neuroscience, Lloyd BuildingTrinity College DublinDublin 2Ireland
  2. 2.Department of Medical Gerontology, Institute of Neuroscience, Lloyd BuildingTrinity College DublinDublin 2Ireland

Personalised recommendations