Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 202, Issue 1, pp 129–145 | Cite as

Identifying sites of saccade amplitude plasticity in humans: transfer of adaptation between different types of saccade

  • J. Johanna Hopp
  • Albert F. Fuchs
Research Article

Abstract

To view different objects of interest, primates use fast, accurate eye movements called saccades. If saccades become inaccurate, the brain adjusts their amplitudes so they again land on target, a process known as saccade adaptation. The different types of saccades elicited in different behavioral circumstances appear to utilize different parts of the oculomotor circuitry. To gain insight into where adaptation occurs in different saccade pathways, we adapted saccades of one type and examined how that adaptation affected or transferred to saccades of a different type. If adaptation of one type of saccade causes a substantial change in the amplitude of another, that adaptation may occur at a site used in the generation of both types of saccade. Alternatively, if adaptation of one type of saccade transfers only partially, or not at all, to another, adaptation occurs at least in part at a location that is not common to the generation of both types of saccade. We produced significant amplitude reductions in memory-guided, delayed, targeting and express saccades by moving the target backward during the saccade. After memory-guided saccades were adapted, the amplitude of express, targeting and delayed saccades exhibited only a partial reduction. In contrast, when express, targeting, or delayed saccades were adapted, amplitude transfer to memory-guided saccades was more substantial. These results, combined with previously published data, suggest that there are at least two sites of adaptation within the saccadic system. One is used communally in the generation of express, targeting, delayed and memory-guided saccades, whereas the other is specific for the generation of memory-guided saccades.

Keywords

Motor learning Saccade gain changes Loci of plasticity Saccade adaptation 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the comments of our colleagues S. Bierer, C.R.S. Kaneko, Y. Kojima, L. Ling, J. Phillips, and R. Soetedjo on an early version of this manuscript. We also thank M. Ibarreta and R. Soetedjo for assistance with the figures. The material presented here was submitted in partial fulfillment of the Ph.D. requirements of the Department of Physiology and Biophysics at the University of Washington. This study was supported by the Vision Training Grant T32 EY07031 (JJH), RO1 EY00745 (AFF), NASA (NNA04CC60G), RR00166 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), and a University of Wisconsin—Stout Faculty Research Initiative Grant. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NCRR or NIH.

References

  1. Abel L, Schmidt D, Dell’Osso L, Daroff R (1978) Saccadic system plasticity in human. Ann Neurol 4:313–318CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alahyane N, Salemme R, Urquizar C, Cotti J, Guillaume A, Vercher J-L, Péllison D (2007) Oculomotor plasticity: are mechanisms of adaptation for reactive and voluntary saccades separate? Brain Res 1135:107–121CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Amador N, Schlag-Reh M, Schlag J (1998) Primate antisaccades. I. Behavioral consequences. J Neurophysiol 80:1775–1786PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Becker W (1989) Metrics. In: Wurtz B, Goldberg M (eds) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 13–67Google Scholar
  5. Bell A, Everling S, Munoz D (2000) Influence of stimulus eccentricity and direction on characteristics of pro- and antisaccades in non-human primates. J Neurophysiol 84:2595–2604PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Braun D, Weber H, Mergner T, Schulte-Monting J (1992) Saccadic reaction times in patients with frontal and parietal lesions. Brain 115:1359–1386CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Catz N, Dicke P, Thier P (2008) Cerebellar-dependent motor learning is based on pruning a Purkinje cell population response. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:7309–7314CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Deng, S, Goldberg, M, Segraves, M, Ungerleider, L, Mishkin, M (1985) The effect of unilateral ablation of the frontal eye fields on the saccadic performance in the monkey. In: Adaptive processes in visual and oculomotor systems. Pergamon, Oxford, pp 201–208Google Scholar
  9. Deubel H (1994) Selective adaptation of intentional and reactive saccades. In: Fuchs A, Brandt Th, Büttner U, Zee D (eds) Contemporary ocular motor and vestibular research: a tribute to David A. Robinson. Thieme, Stuttgart, pp 206–208Google Scholar
  10. Deubel H (1995a) Is saccadic adaptation context-specific? In: Findlay J, Kentridge R, Walker R (eds) Eye movement research: mechanisms, processes and applications. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 177–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Deubel H (1995b) Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements. Vision Res 35:3529–3540CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Deubel H (1999) Separate mechanisms for the adaptive control of reactive, volitional, and memory-guided saccadic eye movements. In: Gopher D, Koriat A (eds) Attention and performance XVII cognitive regulation of performance: interaction of theory and application. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 697–721Google Scholar
  13. Deubel H, Wolf W, Hauske G (1986) Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. Human Neurobiol 5:245–253Google Scholar
  14. Draper N, Smith H (1981) Applied regression analysis, 2nd edn. Wiley, New York, pp 97–98Google Scholar
  15. Edelman J, Goldberg M (2002) Effect of short-term saccadic adaptation on saccades evoked by electrical stimulation of the primate superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 87:1915–1923PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Erkelens C, Hulleman J (1993) Selective adaptation of internally triggered saccades made to visual targets. Exp Brain Res 93:157–164CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fischer B, Weber H (1993) Express saccades and visual attention. Behav Brain Res 16:553–610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fischer B, Weber H, Biscaldi M, Aiple F, Otto P, Stuhr V (1993) Separate populations of visually guided saccades in humans: reaction times and amplitudes. Exp Brain Res 92:528–541CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fisher B, Boch R (1983) Saccadic eye movements after extremely short reaction times. Brain Res 260:21–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fitzgibbon E, Goldberg M, Segraves M (1986) Short term adaptation in the monkey. In: Keller EL, Zee DS (eds) Adaptive processes in visual and oculomotor systems. Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 329–333Google Scholar
  21. Frens M, van Opstal A (1994) Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye movements. Exp Brain Res 100:293–306CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Frens M, van Opstal A (1997) Monkey superior colliculus activity during short-term saccadic adaptation. Brain Res Bull 43:473–483CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Fuchs A, Reiner D, Pong M (1996) Transfer of gain changes from targeting to other types of saccade in the monkey: constraints on possible sites of saccadic gain adaptation. J Neurophysiol 74:2522–2535Google Scholar
  24. Fujita M, Amagai A, Minakawa F, Aoki M (2002) Selective and delay adaptation of human saccades. Cog Brain Res 13:41–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fukushima K, Tanaka S, Williams J, Fukushima J (2005) Voluntary control of saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements in children with learning disorders. Brain Dev 27:579–588CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hopp J, Fuchs A (2002) Investigating the site of human saccadic gain adaptation with targeting and express saccades. Exp Brain Res 144:538–548CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hopp J, Fuchs A (2004) The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 72:27–53CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Kommerell G, Oliver D, Theopold H (1976) Adaptive programming of phasic and tonic components in saccadic eye movements. Invest Ophthal 155:657–660Google Scholar
  29. MacAskill M, Anderson T, Jones R (2002) Adaptive modification of saccade amplitude in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 125:1570–1582CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. McLaughlin S (1967) Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys 2:359–362Google Scholar
  31. Melis B, van Gisbergen J (1996) Short-term adaptation of electrically induced saccades in monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 76:1744–1758PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Munoz D, Broughton J, Goldring J, Armstrong I (1998) Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res 121:391–400CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Agid Y (1991) Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 83:607–617CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Robinson DA (1963) A method of measuring eye movement using a scleral search coil in a magnetic field. IEEE Trans Biomed Elect, BME 10:137–145Google Scholar
  35. Schiller P, Sandell J, Maunsell J (1987) The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 57:1033–1049PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Scudder C, Kaneko C, Fuchs A (2002) The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements: a modern synthesis. Exp Brain Res 142:439–462CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Smit A, van Gisbergen A, Cools A (1987) A parametric analysis of human saccades in different experimental paradigms. Vision Res 27:1745–1762CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Soetedjo R, Kojima Y, Fuchs A (2008) Complex spike activity in the oculomotor vermis: an error signal for saccade motor learning? J Neurophysiol 100:1949–1966CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Straube A, Deubel H, Spuler A, Büttner U (1995) Differential effect of a bilateral deep cerebellar nuclei lesion on externally and internally triggered saccades in humans. Neuroophthalmology 15:67–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Warabi T, Kase M, Kato T (1984) Effect of aging on the accuracy of visually guided saccadic eye movement. Ann Neurol 16:449–454CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. White J, Sparks D, Stanford T (1994) Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors. Vision Res 34:79–92CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhysicsUniversity of Wisconsin-StoutMenomonieUSA
  2. 2.Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Washington National Primate Research CenterUniversity of WashingtonSeattleUSA

Personalised recommendations