Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 196, Issue 4, pp 565–571 | Cite as

Enhanced sensory perception in synaesthesia

Research Note

Abstract

Previous findings imply that synaesthetic experience may have consequences for sensory processing of stimuli that do not themselves trigger synaesthesia. For example, synaesthetes who experience colour show enhanced perceptual processing of colour compared to non-synaesthetes. This study aimed to investigate whether enhanced perceptual processing was a core property of synaesthesia by contrasting tactile and colour sensitivity in synaesthetes who experience either colour, touch, or both touch and colour as evoked sensations. For comparison the performance of non-synaesthetic control subjects was also assessed. There was a relationship between the modality of synaesthetic experience and the modality of sensory enhancement. Synaesthetes who experience colour have enhanced colour sensitivity and synaesthetes who experience touch have enhanced tactile sensitivity. These findings suggest the possibility that a hyper-sensitive concurrent perceptual system is a general property of synaesthesia and are discussed in relation to theories of the condition.

Keywords

Synaesthesia Colour Touch Sensory perception Multisensory processing Cross-modal plasticity 

References

  1. Aghajanian GK, Marek GJ (1999) Serotonin and hallucinogens. Neuropsychopharmacology 21:16S–23SPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Banissy MJ, Ward J (2007) Mirror-touch synaesthesia is linked with empathy. Nat Neurosci 10:815–816PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bargary G, Mitchell KJ (2008) Synaesthesia and cortical connectivity. Trends Neurosci 31:335–342PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barnett KJ, Fox JJ, Molholm S, Kelly SP, Shalgi S, Mitchell KJ, Newell FN (2008) Differences in early sensory-perceptual processing in synaesthesia: a visual evoked potential study. Neuroimage 43:605–613PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blake R, Palmeri TJ, Marois R, Kim CY (2005) On the perceptual reality of synesthetic color. In: Robertson L, Sagiv N (eds) Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 47–73Google Scholar
  6. Blakemore SJ, Bristow D, Bird G, Frith C, Ward J (2005) Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. Brain 128:1571–1583PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen Kadosh R, Henik A (2006) When a line is a number: colour yields magnitude information in a digit-colour synaesthete. Neuroscience 137:3–5PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen Kadosh R, Henik A (2007) Can synaesthesia research inform cognitive science? Trends Cogn Sci 11:177–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen Kadosh R, Walsh V (2006) Cognitive neuroscience: rewired or crosswired brains. Curr Biol 16:962–963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen Kadosh R, Walsh V (2008) Synaesthesia and cortical connections: cause or correlation? Trends Neurosci 31:549–550PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cohen Kadosh R, Cohen Kadosh K, Henik A (2007) The neural correlate of bidirectional synaesthesia: a combined ERP and fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 19:2050–2059PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cohen Kadosh R, Henik A, Catena A, Walsh V, Fuentes LJ (2009) Induced cross-modal synesthetic experience without abnormal neuronal connections. Psychol Sci 20:258–265PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Davidoff J (2001) Language and perceptual categorisation. Trends Cogn Sci 5:382–387PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eagleman DM, Kagan AD, Nelson SS, Sagaram D, Sarma AK (2007) A standardized test battery for the study of synaesthesia. J Neurosci Methods 159:139–145PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goldreich D, Kanics IM (2003) Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness. J Neurosci 23:3439–3445PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Goller A, Otten L, Ward J (2009) Seeing sounds and hearing colours: an event-related potential study of audio-visual synaesthesia. J Cogn Neurosci. doi:10.1162/jocn2009.21134
  17. Grossenbacher PG, Lovelace CT (2001) Mechanisms of synaesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints. Trends Cogn Sci 5:36–41PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hong SW, Blake R (2008) Early visual mechanisms do not contribute to synaesthetic colour experience. Vision Res 48:1018–1026PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Howell DC (2002) Statistical methods for psychology. Thomson Learning, USAGoogle Scholar
  20. Hubbard EM, Ramachandran VS (2005) Neurocognitive mechanisms of synaesthesia. Neuron 48:509–520PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Johnsen KO, Philips JR (1981) Tactile spatial resolution. I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. J Neurophysiol 46:1177–1191Google Scholar
  22. Kaplan E (1991) The receptive field of retinal ganglion cells in cat and monkey. In: Leventhal AG (ed) Vision and visual dysfunction. CRC Press, Boston, pp 10–40Google Scholar
  23. Kauffman T, Théoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Braille character discrimination in blindfolded human subjects. Neuroreport 13:571–574PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kim CY, Blake R, Palmeri TJ (2006) Perceptual interaction between real and synesthetic colors. Cortex 42:195–203PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB (2005) The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:377–401PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Rich AN, Mattingley JB (2002) Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:43–52PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Robertson D, Davies I, Davidoff J (2000) Colour categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. J Exp Psychol 129:369–398Google Scholar
  28. Rouw R, Scholte HS (2007) Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Nat Neurosci 10:792–797PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Saenz M, Koch C (2008) The sound of change: visually-induced auditory synesthesia. Curr Biol 18:650–651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sagiv N, Ward J (2006) Cross-modal interactions: lessons from synaesthesia. Prog Brain Res 155:263–275Google Scholar
  31. Smilek D, Dixon MJ, Cudahy C, Merikle PM (2001) Synaesthetic photisms influence visual perception. J Cogn Neurosci 13:930–936PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Van Boven RW, Johnson K (1994) A psychophysical study of the mechanisms of sensory recovery following nerve injury in humans. Brain 117:149–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Van Boven RW, Hamilton RH, Kauffman T, Keenan P, Pascual-Leone A (2000) Tactile spatial resolution in blind Braille readers. Neurology 54:2230–2236PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Vega-Bermudez F, Johnsen KO (1999) SA1 and RA receptive fields, response variability, and population response mapped with a probe array. J Neurophysiol 81:2701–2710PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Wittenberg GF, Werhahn KJ, Wassermann EM, Herscovitch P, Cohen LG (2004) Functional connectivity between somatosensory and visual cortex in early blind humans. Eur J Neurosci 20:1923–1927PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Yaro C, Ward J (2007) Searching for Shereshevski: what is superior about the memory of synaesthetes? Q J Exp Psychol 60:681–695CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael J. Banissy
    • 1
  • Vincent Walsh
    • 1
  • Jamie Ward
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences, Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of SussexBrightonUK

Personalised recommendations