Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 195, Issue 3, pp 383–392 | Cite as

Response preparation changes during practice of an asynchronous bimanual movement

  • Dana Maslovat
  • Anthony N. Carlsen
  • Romeo Chua
  • Ian M. Franks
Research Article

Abstract

For synchronous bimanual movements, we have shown that a different amplitude can be prepared for each limb in advance and this preparation improves with practice (Maslovat et al. 2008). In the present study, we tested whether an asynchronous bimanual movement can also be prepared in advance and be improved with practice. Participants practiced (160 trials) a discrete bimanual movement in which the right arm led the left by 100 ms in response to an auditory “go” signal (either 80 dB control stimulus or 124 dB startle stimulus). The startle stimulus was used to gauge whether inter-limb timing could be pre-programed. During startle trials, the asynchronous bimanual movement was triggered at early latency suggesting the entire movement could be prepared in advance. However, the triggered movement had a shorter between-arm delay and a temporally compressed within-arm EMG pattern, results that we attribute to increased neural activation caused by the startling stimulus. However, as both startle and control trials improved over time, it does appear response preparation of interval timing can improve with practice.

Keywords

Response preparation Programing Practice Timing Startle 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments for this study go to a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant awarded to Ian M. Franks.

References

  1. Adam JJ, Nieuwenstein JH, Huys R, Paas FGWC, Kingman H, Willems P, Werry M (2000) Control of rapid aimed hand movements: the one-target advantage. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:295–312PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Block RA, Zakay D (1996) Models of psychological time revisited. In: Helfrich H (ed) Time and mind. Hogrefe and Huber Publishers, Gottingen, pp 171–195Google Scholar
  3. Block RA, Zakay D (2008) Timing and remembering the past, the present, and the future. In: Grondin S (ed) Psychology of time. Emerald, Bingley, pp 367–394Google Scholar
  4. Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2003) Startle response is dishabituated during a reaction time task. Exp Brain Res 152:510–518PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2004a) Can prepared responses be stored subcortically? Exp Brain Res 159:301–309PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2004b) Prepared movements are elicited early by startle. J Mot Behav 36:253–264PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carlsen AN, Dakin C, Chua R, Franks IM (2007) Startle produces early response latencies that are distinct from stimulus intensity effects. Exp Brain Res 176:199–205PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carlsen AN, Chua R, Dakin DJ, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2008) Startle reveals an absence of advance motor programming in a Go/No-go task. Neurosci Lett 434:61–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2009) Differential effects of startle on reaction time for finger and arm movements. J Neurophysiol 101:306–314PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL, Agarwal GC (1989) Organizing principles for single-joint movements. II. A speed-sensitive strategy. J Neurophysiol 62:358–368PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Coull JT, Nobre AC (2008) Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:137–144PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cressman EK, Carlsen AN, Chua R, Franks IM (2006) Temporal uncertainty does not affect response latencies of movements produced during startle reactions. Exp Brain Res 171:278–282PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Donders FC (1969) On the speed of mental processes. In: Koster WG (ed and trans) Attention and performance II. North Holland, Amsterdam (original work published in 1868)Google Scholar
  14. Fischman MG, Lim CH (1991) Influence of extended practice on programming time, movement time, and transfer in simple target-striking responses. J Mot Behav 23:39–50PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Gottlieb GL, Corcos DM, Agarwal GC (1989) Organizing principles for single-joint movements. I. A speed-insensitive strategy. J Neurophysiol 62:342–357PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Gruber RP, Block RA (2005) Effects of caffeine on prospective duration judgements of various intervals depend on task difficult. Hum Psychopharmacol 20:275–285PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Khan MA, Garry MI, Franks IM (1999) The effect of target size and inertial load on the control of rapid aiming movements. Exp Brain Res 124:151–158PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Khan MA, Lawrence GP, Buckolz E, Franks IM (2006) Programming strategies for rapid aiming movements under simple and choice reaction time conditions. Q J Exp Psychol A 59:524–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Klapp ST (1995) Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: the role of practice. J Exp Psychol Human 21:1015–1027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Klapp ST (1996) Reaction time analysis of central motor control. In: Zelaznik HN (ed) Advances in motor learning and control. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 13–35Google Scholar
  21. Klapp ST (2003) Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: action as a sequence of chunks. J Mot Behav 35:135–150PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Macar F, Vidal F (2004) Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: a review. J Pyschophysiol 18:89–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. MacKinnon CD, Bissig D, Chiusano J, Miller E, Rudnick L, Jager C, Zhang Y, Mille M-L, Rogers MW (2007) Preparation of anticipatory postural adjustments prior to stepping. J Neurophysiol 97:4368–4379PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Maslovat D, Carlsen AN, Ishimoto R, Chua R, Franks IM (2008) Response preparation changes following practice of an asymmetrical bimanual movement. Exp Brain Res 190:239–249PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Meck WH (1996) Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cogn Brain Res 3:227–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pashler H (1994) Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull 116:220–244PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Penton-Voak IS, Edwards H, Percival A, Wearden JH (1996) Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:307–320PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Queralt A, Valls-Solé J, Castellote JM (2008) The effects of a startle on the sit-to-stand manoeuvre. Exp Brain Res 185:603–609PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Robertson SD, Zelaznik HN, Lantero DA, Bojczyk KG, Spencer RM, Doffin JG, Schneidt T (1999) Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: evidence against a single timing process for motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1316–1330PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Rothwell JC (2006) The startle reflex, voluntary movement, and the reticulospinal tract. In: Cruccu G, Hallett M (eds) Brainstem function and dysfunction. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 221–229Google Scholar
  31. Sherwood DE (1994) Hand preference, practice order, and spatial assimilations in rapid bimanual movement. J Mot Behav 26:123–134PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Sherwood DE (2006) Intermovement interval and spatial assimilation effects in sequential bimanual and unimanual movement. Hum Mov Sci 25:145–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sherwood DE, Canabal MY (1988) The effect of practice on the control of sequential and simultaneous actions. Hum Perform 1:237–260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Siegmund GP, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ (2001) Startle response of human neck muscles sculpted by readiness to perform ballistic head movements. J Physiol 535:289–300PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stucchi N, Viviani P (1993) Cerebral dominance and asynchrony between bimanual two-dimensional movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:1200–1220PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Summers JJ, Maeder S, Hiraga CY, Alexander JRM (2008) Coordination dynamics and attentional costs of continuous and discontinuous bimanual circle drawing movements. Hum Mov Sci 27:823–837PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Swinnen SP, Wenderoth N (2004) Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill. Trends Cogn Sci 8:18–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Swinnen SP, Jardin K, Meulenbroek R (1996) Between-limb asynchronies during bimanual coordination: effects of manual dominance and attentional cueing. Neuropsychologia 34:1203–1213PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Taatgen NA, van Rijn H, Anderson J (2007) An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: the role of cognition, attention, and learning. Psychol Rev 114:577–598PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Triesman M (1963) Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: implications for a model of the internal clock. Psychophysiol Monogr 76:1–31Google Scholar
  41. Valls-Solé J, Solé A, Valldeoriola F, Muñoz E, Gonzalez LE, Tolosa ES (1995) Reaction time and acoustic startle in normal human subjects. Neurosci Lett 195:97–100PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Valls-Solé J, Rothwell JC, Goulart F, Cossu G, Muñoz E (1999) Patterned ballistic movements triggered by a startle in healthy humans. J Physiol 516.3:931–938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Vindras P, Viviani P (2005) Planning short pointing sequences. Exp Brain Res 160:141–153PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zelaznik HN, Spencer RM, Doffin JG (2000) Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: further evidence against timing as a general-purpose ability. J Mot Behav 32:193–199PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Zelaznik HN, Spencer RM, Ivry RT (2002) Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:575–588PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dana Maslovat
    • 1
  • Anthony N. Carlsen
    • 1
  • Romeo Chua
    • 1
  • Ian M. Franks
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Human KineticsUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations