Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 192, Issue 3, pp 521–525 | Cite as

Planning actions in autism

  • Maddalena Fabbri-Destro
  • Luigi Cattaneo
  • Sonia Boria
  • Giacomo Rizzolatti
Research Article

Abstract

It has been suggested that the deficit in understanding others’ intention in autism depends on a malfunctioning of the mirror system. This malfunction could be due either to a deficit of the basic mirror mechanism or to a disorganization of chained action organization on which the mirror understanding of others’ intention is based. Here we tested this last hypothesis investigating the kinematics of intentional actions. Children with autism and typically developing children (TD) were asked to execute two actions consisting each of three motor acts: the first was identical in both actions while the last varied for its difficulty. The result showed that, unlike in TD children, in children with autism the kinematics of the first motor act was not modulated by the task difficulty. This finding strongly supports the notion that children with autism have a deficit in chaining motor acts into a global action.

Keywords

Autism Motor acts Action planning Mirror neuron system 

References

  1. Altschuler EL, Vankov A, Wang V, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA (1997) Person see, person, poster session. Presented at the 27th annual meeting of the society for neuroscience, New Orleans, November 1997Google Scholar
  2. Boria S, Fabbri-Destro M, Cattaneo L, Sparaci L, Sinigaglia C, Santelli E, Cossu G, Rizzolatti G (2008) ‘What’ and ‘why’ in autism (submitted)Google Scholar
  3. Cattaneo L, Fabbri-Destro M, Boria S, Pieraccini C, Monti A, Cossu G, Rizzolatti G (2007) Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:17825–17830PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cochin S, Barthelemy C, Roux S, Martineau J (1999) Observation and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified electroencephalography. Eur J Neurosci 11:1839–1842PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Iacoboni M (2006) Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci 9:28–30PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G (2005) Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308:662–667PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fuster JM (2002) Cortex and mind: unifying cognition. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager-Flusberg H (2006) Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism. Cereb Cortex 16:1276–1282PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hamilton AF, Brindley RM, Frith U (2007) Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: how valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia 45:1859–1868PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G (1998) Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:15061–15065PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Johnson-Frey SH, McCarty M, Keen R (2004) Reaching beyond spatial perception: effects of intended future actions on visually-guided prehension. Visual Cogn 11:371–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kanner L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child 2:217–250Google Scholar
  13. Lord C, Rutter M, Di Lovore PC, Risi S (2005) Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Organizzazioni Speciali, FlorenceGoogle Scholar
  14. Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M, Athenes S, Dugas C (1987) Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Can J Psychol 41:365–378PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Martineau J, Cochin S, Magne R, Barthelemy C (2008) Impaired cortical activation in autistic children: is the mirror neuron system involved? Int J Psychophysiol 68:35–40PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Nishitani N, Avikainen S, Hari R (2004) Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Ann Neurol 55:558–562PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Oberman LM, Hubbard EM, McCleery JP, Altschuler EL, Ramachandran VS, Pineta JA (2005) EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:190–198PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA (2008) Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: the mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 46:1558–1565PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Raven JC (1984) Coloured progressive matrices. Organizzazioni Speciali, FlorenceGoogle Scholar
  20. Rubini V, Padovani F (1986) Wechsler intelligence scale for children revised. Organizzazioni Speciali, FlorenceGoogle Scholar
  21. Tanji J, Hoshi E (2008) Role of the prefrontal cortex n executive behavior control. Physiol Rev 88:37–57PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Théoret H, Halligan E, Kobayashi M, Fregni F, Tager-Flusberg H, Pascual-Leone A (2005) Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Curr Biol 15:R84–R85PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Williams JH, Whiten A, Suddendorf T, Perrett DI (2001) Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:287–295PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Maddalena Fabbri-Destro
    • 1
    • 2
  • Luigi Cattaneo
    • 1
  • Sonia Boria
    • 1
  • Giacomo Rizzolatti
    • 1
  1. 1.Dipartimento di NeuroscienzeUniversità di ParmaParmaItaly
  2. 2.Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche e Terapie AvanzateUniversità di FerraraFerraraItaly

Personalised recommendations