Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 189, Issue 3, pp 361–372 | Cite as

TauG-guidance of transients in expressive musical performance

  • Benjaman Schogler
  • Gert-Jan Pepping
  • David N. Lee
Research Article

Abstract

The sounds in expressive musical performance, and the movements that produce them, offer insight into temporal patterns in the brain that generate expression. To gain understanding of these brain patterns, we analyzed two types of transient sounds, and the movements that produced them, during a vocal duet and a bass solo. The transient sounds studied were inter-tone f 0(t)-glides (the continuous change in fundamental frequency, f 0(t), when gliding from one tone to the next), and attack intensity-glides (the continuous rise in sound intensity when attacking, or initiating, a tone). The temporal patterns of the inter-tone f 0(t)-glides and attack intensity-glides, and of the movements producing them, all conformed to the mathematical function, τ G(t) (called tauG), predicted by General Tau Theory, and assumed to be generated in the brain. The values of the parameters of the τ G(t) function were modulated by the performers when they modulated musical expression. Thus the τ G(t) function appears to be a fundamental of brain activity entailed in the generation of expressive temporal patterns of movement and sound.

Keywords

Brain Movement control Acoustics Tau coupling Musical expression General Tau Theory 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The research was supported by a grant from the University of Minnesota, and the writing by a Leverhulme Trust Fellowship to DNL. We thank P. Biggs, E. Ward, B. Harvey, and J. Scriven who helped carry out the singing and bass playing experiments for their Senior Honours dissertations in Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 2004. We thank the Speech Science Research Centre, Queen Margaret University College, Edinburgh where the singing experiments were carried out with the help of Nigel Hewlett and colleagues.

References

  1. Bernstein NA (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. Boersma P, Weenink D (2000) Praat 3.9: a system for doing phonetics by computer. http://www.praat.org
  3. Clarke EF (1988) Generative principles in music performance. In: Sloboda J (ed) Generative processes in music. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 1–27Google Scholar
  4. Clynes M (1973) Sentics: biocybernetics of emotion communication. Ann NY Acad Sci 220:55–131PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Craig CM, Lee DN (1999) Neonatal control of nutritive sucking pressure: evidence for an intrinsic τ-guide. Exp Br Res 124:371–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Craig CM, Delay D, Grealy MA, Lee DN (2000a) Guiding the swing in golf putting. Nature 405:295–296PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Craig CM, Grealy MA, Lee DN (2000b) Detecting motor abnormalities in pre-term infants. Exp Br Res 131:359–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Craig CM, Pepping G-J, Grealy MA (2005) Intercepting beats in pre-designated target zones. Exp Br Res 165:490–504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fourcin AJ (1981) Laryngographic assessment of phonatory function. In: Ludlow CL, Hart MO (eds) Proceedings of the conference of the assessment of vocal pathology, ASHA reports No. 11, pp 116–127Google Scholar
  10. Galembo A, Askenfelt A, Cuddy L, Russo F (2001) Effects of relative phase on pitch and timbre in piano bass range. J Acoust Soc Am 110:1649–1666PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  12. Hanna JL (1979) To dance is human. A theory of nonverbal communication. University of Texas Press, AustinGoogle Scholar
  13. Janata P, Grafton T (2003) Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music. Nat Neurosci 6:682–687PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Juslin PN, Sloboda JA (2001) Music and emotion. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  15. Keysers C, Kohler E, Umilta MA, Nanetti L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2003) Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition. Exp Br Res 153:628–636CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lee DN (1998) Guiding movement by coupling taus. Ecol Psychol 10:221–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lee DN (2005) Tau in action in development. In: Rieser JJ, Lockman JJ, Nelson CA (eds) Action as an organizer of learning and development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 3–49Google Scholar
  18. Lee DN, Craig CM, Grealy MA (1999) Sensory and intrinsic coordination of movement. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:2029–2035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lee DN, Georgopoulos AP, Clark MJ, Craig CM, Port NL (2001) Guiding contact by coupling the taus of gaps. Exp Br Res 139:151–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lee DN, Georgopoulos AP, Lee TM, Pepping G-J (2008) A neural formula that guides movement (submitted)Google Scholar
  21. Meister IG, Peretz I, Gagnon L, Hebert S, Macoir J (2004) Singing in the brain: insights from cognitive neuropsychology. Music Percept 21:373–390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mitchell RW, Gallaher MC (2001) Embodying music: matching music and dance in memory. Music Percept 19:65–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Panksepp J, Bernatzky G (2002) Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behav Processes 60:133–155PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Peretz I, Zatorre RJ (2005) Brain organization for music processing. Annu Rev Psychol 56:89–114PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Popescu M, Otsuka A, Ioannides AA (2004) Dynamics of brain activity in motor and frontal cortical areas during music listening: a magnetoencephalographic study. Neuroimage 21:1622–1638PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Ramani N, Miall RC (2004) A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others. Nat Neurosci 7:85–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Repp BH (1990) Patterns of expressive timing in performances of a Beethoven minuet by 19 famous pianists. J Acoust Soc Am 88:622–641PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Repp BH (1995) Expressive timing in Schumann’s “Träumerei”: an analysis of performances by graduate student pianists. J Acoust Soc Am 98:2413–2427PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schlaug G (2001) The brain of musicians. A model for functional and structural adaptation. Ann NY Acad Sci 930:281–299PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Scholes PA (1960) The Oxford companion to music. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  31. Timmers R, Ashley R, Desain P, Heijink H (2000) The influence of musical context on tempo rubato. J New Music Res 29:131–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Todd NP (1994) The kinematics of musical expression. J Acoust Soc Am 97:1940–1949CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Trehub SE (2003) The developmental origins of musicality. Nat Neurosci 6:669–673PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Trevarthen C (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. In: Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae Special Issue 1999–2000, pp 157–213Google Scholar
  35. Von Hofsten C (1983) Catching skills in infancy. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 9:75–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Zatorre RJ (2003) Music and the brain. Neurosci Music 999:4–14Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Benjaman Schogler
    • 1
  • Gert-Jan Pepping
    • 1
    • 2
  • David N. Lee
    • 1
  1. 1.Perception-Movement-Action Research Centre, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences and Moray House School of EducationUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  2. 2.Center for Human Movement SciencesUniversity of GroningenGroningenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations