Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 188, Issue 2, pp 237–247

Head, arm and trunk coordination during reaching in children

  • H. Sveistrup
  • S. Schneiberg
  • P. A. McKinley
  • B. J. McFadyen
  • M. F. Levin
Research Article

Abstract

During postural and locomotor tasks, the orientation of the head with respect to space is maintained in order to serve as an egocentric reference value for maintaining balance. In young children during locomotor tasks, task difficulty determines the coordination of movements between head–trunk segments: the more difficult the task, the more the child limits the head on trunk movement (“en bloc”) rather than letting the head move freely in space. For reaching tasks, however, there are no data about the development and maturation of coordination between the head and trunk movements and when the pattern of coordination is considered mature. The goal of this study was to characterize the development of head–trunk coordination during reaching from a sitting position in typically developing children. Forty-four typically-developing (TD) children aged from 2.8 to 11.8 years and six healthy adults participated. Children were divided into five groups (G1–G5) according to their age: 2–3, 4–5, 6–7, 8–9 and 10–11 years old. The task involved reaching towards and grasping a piece of food in the younger group or a wooden block in the older children and adults with the dominant hand, adequate to the grip size of each participant, and returning it to the mouth area to simulate self-feeding. The object was placed in line with the midline of the body at three different distances from the trunk according to the participant’s arm length (two within and one beyond arm’s length). Rotational movements of the head and trunk in three planes; yaw, roll and pitch, were recorded using three-dimensional tracking systems (Optotrak, Northern Digital, Model 3010 or Ariel Performance Analysis System). The variables analysed were relative head and trunk angle, absolute head and trunk angle, the anchoring index (AI) and initial direction of head and trunk rotation (direction index: DI). Patterns of head–trunk coupling were different along different axes of rotation and across groups. For the AI, a head-stabilized-on-trunk (HST) or “en bloc” pattern was observed with approximately the same frequency as a head-stabilized-in-space (HSS) pattern in the youngest children in the yaw plane for reaches within arm’s length. In all other planes and for reaches of all distances, a HSS pattern was evident in the youngest children and remained consistent across the groups of children. Compared to the children, adult reaching was characterised by fixed head–trunk coordination (HST) in the roll plane at all reach distances, and greater decoupling in yaw plane motion for the two closest distances. There were no age-related differences in the pitch plane strategy which was mainly HSS. The DI patterns matured by 2–3 or 4–5 years of age, except for reaches to T1 in the pitch plane. In addition, in the roll plane, there was evidence of a two-step maturation that was not complete until adulthood. Maturation of strategies used to stabilize the head and trunk relative to each other and to the reaching arm differ across movement planes for a seated reaching task. Our data suggest that different aspects of  head and trunk coordination during reaching movement mature at different rates, like for locomotor tasks previously described, and that the maturation follows a non-chronological and protracted course. These results can serve as a comparative database with which to contrast head and trunk coordination in children with movement disorders. However, in terms of typical development, these data should be considered specific for the task studied and may not reflect general principles of motor development.

Keywords

Motor control Reaching Grasping Head trunk coordination 

References

  1. Ada L, Canning CG, Carr JH, Kilbreath SL, Shepherd RB (1994) Task specific training of reaching and manipulation. In: Bennett KMB, Castiello U (eds) Insights into the Reach and Grasp Movement. Amsterdam, pp 234–265Google Scholar
  2. Assaiante C (1998) Development of locomotor balance control in health children. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:527–532PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Assaiante C (2000) Construction du répertoire des référentiels posturaux: maturation et adaptation au cours de l’enfance. Habilitation a Dirigé des Recherches. Université de la Méditerranée, Aix-Marseille IIGoogle Scholar
  4. Assaiante C, Amblard B (1993) Ontogenesis of head stabilization in space during locomotion in children: influence of visual cues. Exp Brain Res 93:499–515PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Assaiante C, Amblard B (1995) An ontogenetic model for the sensorimotor organization of balance control in humans. Hum Mov Sci 14:13–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Assaiante C, Amblard B (1996) Visual factors in the child’s gait: effects on locomotor skills. Percept Mot Skills 83(3 Pt 1):1019–1041PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Assaiante C, Mallau S, Viel A, Jover M, Schmitz C (2005) Development of postural control in healthy children: a functional approach. Neural Plast 12:109–118PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Assaiante C, McKinley PA, Amblard B (1997) Head–trunk coordination during hops using one or two feet in children and adults. J Vestib Res 7(2–3):145–160PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bernstein NA (1967) The coordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  10. Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C (1998) Eye, head and trunk control: the foundation for manual development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22(4):515–520PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Berthoz A, Pozzo T (1988) Intermittent head stabilization during postural and locomotory tasks in humans. In: Clarac F (ed) Posture and gait; development adaptation and modulation. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 189–198Google Scholar
  12. Breniere Y, Bril B (1998) Development of postural control of gravity forces in children during the first 5 years of walking. Exp Brain Res 121(3):255–262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Casimiro L, Sveistrup H (2001) Feedforward postural adjustments associated with a goal-directed reach produced by infants in the sitting and standing positions. In: Duysens J, Smits-Engelsman BCM, Kingma H (eds) Control of posture and gait. Solvay, Brussels, pp 23–26Google Scholar
  14. Chari VR, Kirby RL (1986) Lower-limb influence on sitting balance while reaching forward. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 67:730–733PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crosbie J, Herbert RD, Bridson JT (1997) Intersegmental dynamics of standing from sitting. Clin Biomech 12:227–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Crovitz HF, Zener K (1962) A group test for assessing hand and eye dominance. Am J Psych 75:271–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dellen T van, Kalverboer AF (1984) Single movement control and information processing, a developmental study. Behav Brain Res 12:237–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Deutsch KM, Newell KM (2004) Changes in the structure of children’s isometric force variability with practice. J Exp Child Psychol 88:319–333PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. DiFabio RP, Emasithi A (1997) Aging and the mechanisms underlying head and postural control during voluntary motion. Phys Ther 77:458–475Google Scholar
  20. Elliott JM, Connolly KJ (1984) A classification of manipulative hand movements. Dev Med Child Neurol 26:283–296PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Forssberg H, Eliasson AC, Kinoshita H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991) Development of human precision grip. I: basic coordination of force. Exp Brain Res 85(2):451–457PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Forssberg H, Kinoshita H, Eliasson AC, Johansson RS, Westling G, Gordon AM (1992) Development of human precision grip. II: anticipatory control of isometric forces targeted for object’s weight. Exp Brain Res 90:393–398PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Haas G, Diener HC, Rapp H, Dichgans J (1989) Development of feedback and feedforward control of upright stance. Dev Med Child Neurol 31:481–488PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hay L (1978) Accuracy of children on an open-loop pointing task. Percept Mot Skills 47:1079–1082PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hay L, Redon C (1999) Feedforward versus feedback control in children and adults subjected to a postural disturbance. Exp Brain Res 125:153–162PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hay L, Redon C (2001) Development of postural adaptation to arm raising. Exp Brain Res 139:224–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hay L (1990) Developmental changes in eye-hand coordination behaviors: preprogramming versus feedback control. In: Bard C, Fleury M, Hay L (eds) Development of eye-hand coordination across the lifespan. Univ S Carolina Press, Columbia, pp 217–244Google Scholar
  28. Huttenlocher J, Newcombe N, Sandberg EH (1994) The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognit Psychol 27:115–147PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Levin MF, Schneiberg S, Sveistrup H, McKinley P, McFadyen BJ (2003) Variability, the key to motor skill acquisition in children. In: Progress in motor control IV, Caen, AugustGoogle Scholar
  30. McKenzie BE, Skouteris H, Day RH, Hartman B, Yonas A (1993) Effective action by infants to contact objects by reaching and leaning. Child Dev 64:415–429PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Michaelsen S, Luta A, Roby-Brami A, Levin MF (2001) Effect of trunk restraint on the recovery of reaching movements in hemiparetic patients. Stroke 32:1875–1883PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rochat P (1995) Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults. J Exp Child Psychol 59:317–333PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Roncesvalles MN, Schmitz C, Zedka M, Assaiante C, Woollacott M (2005) From egocentric to exocentric spatial orientation: the development of postural control in bi-manual and trunk inclination tasks. J Mot Behav 37:404–416PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schellekens JMH, Kalverboer AF, Scholten CA (1984) The microstructure of tapping movements in children. J Mot Behav 16:20–39PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Schmitz C, Assaiante C (2002) Developmental sequence in the acquisition of anticipation during a new co-ordination in a bimanual load-lifting task in children. Neurosci Lett 330(3):215–218PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schmitz C, Martin N, Assaiante C (1999) Development of anticipatory postural adjustments in a bimanual load-lifting task in children. Exp Brain Res 126:200–204PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schmitz C, Martin N, Assaiante C (2002) Building anticipatory postural adjustment during childhood: a kinematic and electromyographic analysis of unloading in children from 4 to 8 years of age. Exp Brain Res 142:354–364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Schneiberg S, Sveistrup H, McFadyen B, McKinley P, Levin MF (2002) Development of coordination for reaching in children. Exp Brain Res 146:142–154PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schneiberg S, Chen P, Sveistrup H, McKinley P, McFadyen BJ, Levin MF (2003) Head and trunk coordination during reach to grasp movement in children. In: Progress in motor control IV, Caen, AugustGoogle Scholar
  40. Schneiberg S, Lamarre C, Bibeau A, Gendron S, Bilodeau N, Levin M.F (2004) Kinematic patterns during reach and grasp movement in children with mild cerebral palsy before and after constraint induced therapy (CIT). Abstract. American  Academy of Cerebral Palsy and Developmental Medicine, Los Angeles, SeptGoogle Scholar
  41. Thelen E, Spencer JP (1998) Postural control during reaching in young infants: a dynamic systems approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:507–514PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Vallis L, McFadyen BJ (2005) Children use different anticipatory control strategies than adults to circumvent an obstacle in the travel path. Exp Brain Res 167:119–127PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. van der Fits IB, Hadders-Algra M (1998) The development of postural response patterns during reaching in healthy infants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:521–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. van der Fits IB, Klip AWJ, van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M (1999a) Postural adjustments during spontaneous and goal-directed arm movements in the first half year of life. Behav Brain Res 106:75–90PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. van der Fits IB, Otten E, Klip AW, Van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M (1999b) The development of postural adjustments during reaching in 6- to 18-month-old infants. Evidence for two transitions. Exp Brain Res 126:517–528PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. van der Heide JC, Otten B, van Eykern LA, Hadders-Algra M (2003) Development of postural adjustments during reaching in sitting children. Exp Brain Res 151:32–45PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Von Hofsten C, Woollacott MH (1989) Postural preparations for reaching in 9 months old infants. Neurosci Abstr 15:1199Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • H. Sveistrup
    • 1
  • S. Schneiberg
    • 2
    • 3
  • P. A. McKinley
    • 2
    • 3
  • B. J. McFadyen
    • 4
    • 5
  • M. F. Levin
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Faculty of Health SciencesUniversity of OttawaOttawaCanada
  2. 2.Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation (CRIR)MontrealCanada
  3. 3.School of  Physical and Occupational TherapyMcGill UniversityMontrealCanada
  4. 4.CIRRISQuebec cityCanada
  5. 5.Laval UniversityQuebec cityCanada

Personalised recommendations