Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 185, Issue 4, pp 623–633 | Cite as

Effect of tactile stimulus frequency on time perception: the role of working memory

  • Mohammad Ali KhoshnoodiEmail author
  • Rouzbeh Motiei-Langroudi
  • Mohsen Omrani
  • Mathew E. Diamond
  • Abdol Hossein Abbassian
Research Article

Abstract

In most models of interval timing, there is a central clock, which is considered to be highly protected from the effects of external stimuli. However, many studies have reported such effects and different theories are proposed to explain the observations. These include the effect of arousal, attention sharing, memory load and information processing on central clock as well as change in the speed of the pacemaker. In this study, we used regular vibro-tactile stimuli with different frequencies in a “duration reproduction task” to investigate the effect of stimulus content on interval timing. Results showed that subjects overestimated the duration as a function of test stimulus frequency. A significant correlation between increasing the test frequency and overestimation of subjective time was observed. We further investigated the effect of blank and filled gaps with various durations on time estimation. Analysis revealed that regardless of gap duration, subjective time increased in the filled gap condition, compared to the blank gap. This effect was independent from contextual stimuli and correlated to the mean number of stimuli during the temporal interval rather than rate of stimulus presentation.

Keywords

Time perception Stimulus content Central clock Gap Working memory 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to R. Kiani, S. R. Afraz , A. Zandvakili, I. Kurtzer and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive review on this manuscript. We would also like to thank B. Babadi for his helpful comments and helps on the model. The authors would especially thank Prof. R. Ivry for his insightful comments on this work.

References

  1. Block RA, Reed MA (1978) Remembered duration: evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. J Exp Psychol (Hum Learn) 4:656–665CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Block RA, Zakay D (1997) Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychon Bull Rev 4:184–197Google Scholar
  3. Brown SW, Boltz MG (2002) Attentional processes in time perception: effects of mental workload and event structure. J Exp Psychol (Hum Learn) 28:600–615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2000) Timing for the absence of a stimulus: The gap paradigm reversed. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:305–322PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2002) Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. Behav Neurosci 116:291–297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Buhusi CV, Sasaki A, Meck WH (2002) Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). J Comp Psychol 116:381–390PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cantor NE, Thomas EA (1977) Control of attention in the processing of temporal and spatial information in complex visual patterns. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 3:243–250PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Catania AC (1970) Reinforcement schedules and psychophysical judgments: A study of some temporal properties of behavior. In: Schoenfeld WN (ed) The theory of reinforcement schedules, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp 162–197Google Scholar
  10. Creelman CD (1962) Human discrimination of auditory stimuli. J Acoust Soc Am 34:582–593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Church RM, Miller KD, Meck WH (1991) Symmetrical and asymmetrical sources of variance in temporal generalization. Learn Behav 19:207–214Google Scholar
  12. Eagleman DM, Tse PU, Buonomano DV, Janssen P, Nobre AC, Holcombe AO (2005) Time and the brain: how subjective time relates to neural time. J Neurosci 25:10369–10371PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fortin C, Masse N (2000) Expecting a break in time estimation: Attentional time-sharing without concurrent processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1788–1796PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fortin C, Bedard MC, Champagne J (2005) Timing during interruptions in timing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:276–288PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fraisse P (1984) Perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev Psychol 35:1–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frankenhauser M (1959) Estimation of time. Almqvist & Wiksell, StockholmGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibbon J (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and weber’s law in animal timing. Psychol Rev 84:279–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gibbon J, Church RM (1984) Sources of variance in an information processing theory of timing. In: Roitblat HL, Bever TG, Terrace HS (eds) Animal cognition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 465–488Google Scholar
  19. Gibbon J, Church RM, Meck WH (1984) Scalar timing in memory. In: Gibbon J, Allan L (eds) Timing and time perception, New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 52–77Google Scholar
  20. Gibbon J, Church RM (1990) Representation of time. Cognition 37:23–54PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grondin S (1993) Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals. Percept Psychophys 54:383–394PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Grondin S (2001) From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychol Bull 127:22–44PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harris JA, Harris IM, Diamond ME (2001) The topography of tactile working memory. J Neurosci 21:8262–8269PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Hicks RE, Miller GW, Kinsbourne M (1976) Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed. Am J Psychol 89:719–730PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ivry RB, Spencer RM (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:225–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Killeen PR, Fetterman JG (1988) A behavioral theory of timing. Psychol Rev 95:274–295PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Macar F, Grondin S, Casini L (1994) Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Mem Cognit 22:673–686PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Mauk MD, Buonomano DV (2004) The neural basis of temporal processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:304–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Meck WH (1984) Attentional bias between modalities: Effect on the internal clock, memory, and decision stages used in animal time discrimination. In: Gibbon J, Allan L (eds) Timing and time perception, vol 423, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 528–541Google Scholar
  30. Mulligan RM, Schiffman HR (1979) Temporal experience as a function of organization in memory. Bull Psychon Soc 14:417–420Google Scholar
  31. Ornstein RE (1969) On the experience of time. Penguin, MiddlesexGoogle Scholar
  32. Penton-Voak IS, Edwards H, Percival A, Wearden JH (1996) Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:307–320PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Predebon J (1996) The relationship between the number of presented stimuli and prospective duration estimates: the effect of concurrent task activity. Psychon Bull Rev 3:376–379Google Scholar
  34. Rakitin BC, Gibbon J, Penney TB, Malapani C, Hinton SC, Meck WH (1998) Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 24:15–33PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Thomas EAC, Cantor NE (1978) Interdependence between the processing of temporal and non-temporal information. In: Requin J (ed) Attention and performance, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 43–62Google Scholar
  36. Thomas EAC, Weaver WB (1975) Cognitive processing and time perception. Percept Psychophys 17:363–367Google Scholar
  37. Treisman M (1963) Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychol Monogr 77:1–31PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Treisman M, Brogan D (1992) Time perception and the internal clock: Effects of visual flicker on the temporal oscillator. Eur J Cognit Psychol 4:41–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Treisman M, Faulkner A, Naish PL, Brogan D (1990) The internal clock: evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception 19:705–743PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tremblay S, Fortin C (2003) Break expectancy in duration discrimination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:823–831PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tse PU, Rivest J, Intriligator J, Cavanagh P (2004) Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Percept Psychophys 66:1171–1189PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Zakay D (1993) Time estimation methods: Do they influence prospective duration estimates? Perception 22:91–101PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Zakay D (2000) Gating or switching? Gating is a better model of prospective timing (A response to “Switching or Gating?” by Lejeune). Behav Processes 52:63–69PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zakay D, Block RA (2004) Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 64:319–328Google Scholar
  45. Zakay D, Tsal Y (1989) Awareness of attention allocation and time estimation accuracy. Bull Psychon Soc 27:209–210Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mohammad Ali Khoshnoodi
    • 1
    Email author
  • Rouzbeh Motiei-Langroudi
    • 1
  • Mohsen Omrani
    • 1
  • Mathew E. Diamond
    • 2
  • Abdol Hossein Abbassian
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Cognitive SciencesInstitute for Studies in Theoretical Physics and Mathematics (IPM)TehranIran
  2. 2.Cognitive Neuroscience SectorInternational School for Advanced Studies (SISSA)TriesteItaly

Personalised recommendations