Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 184, Issue 2, pp 233–248 | Cite as

Interval timing and Parkinson’s disease: heterogeneity in temporal performance

  • Hugo Merchant
  • Monica Luciana
  • Catalina Hooper
  • Stacy Majestic
  • Paul Tuite
Research Article

Abstract

Interval timing deficiencies in Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients have been a matter of debate. Here we test the possibility of PD heterogeneity as a source for this discrepancy. Temporal performance of PD patients and control subjects was assessed during two interval tapping tasks and during a categorization task of time intervals. These tasks involved temporal processing of intervals in the hundreds of milliseconds range; however, they also covered a wide range of behavioral contexts, differing in their perceptual, decision-making, memory, and execution requirements. The results showed the following significant findings. First, there were two clearly segregated subgroups of PD patients: one with high temporal variability in the three timing tasks, and another with a temporal variability that did not differ substantially from control subjects. In contrast, PD patients with high and low temporal variability showed similar perceptual, decision-making, memory, and execution performance in a set of control tasks. Second, a slope analysis, designed to dissociate time-dependent from time-independent sources of variation, revealed that the increase in variability in this group of PD patients was mainly due to an increment in the variability associated with the timing mechanism. Third, while the control subjects showed significant correlations in performance variability across tasks, PD patients, and particularly those with high temporal variability, did not show such task correlations. Finally, the results showed that dopaminergic treatment restored the correlation effect in PD patients, producing a highly significant correlation between the inter-task variability. Altogether, these results indicate that a subpopulation of PD patients shows a strong disruption in temporal processing in the hundreds of milliseconds range. These findings are discussed in terms of the role of dopamine as a tuning element for the synchronization of temporal processing across different behavioral contexts in PD patients.

Keywords

Interval timing Time perception Time production Parkinson’s disease Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease 

Abbreviations

PD

Parkinson’s disease

UPDRS

Unified Parkinson disease rating scale

MTT

Multiple tap task

STT

Single tap task

CAT

Interval categorization task

SD

Standard deviation

BGTCP

Basal ganglia-thalamocortical pathway

References

  1. Artieda J, Pastor MA, Lacruz F, Obeso JA (1992) Temporal discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115:199–210PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandini F, Pierantozzi M, Bodis-Wollner I (2002) The visuo-cognitive and motor effect of amantadine in non-caucasian patients with Parkinson’s disease. A clinical and electrophysiological study. J Neural Transm 109:41–51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beck AT, Ward C, Mendelson M (1961) Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561–571PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Hopkins BJ (1998) Verbal learning test—revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test–retest reliability. Clin Neuropsychol 12:43–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A (2001) Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb Cortex 11:825–836PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown P (2003) Oscillatory nature of human basal ganglia activity: relationship to the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease. Mov Dis 18:357–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2002) Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. Behav Neurosci 116:291–297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buonomano DV, Karmarkar UR (2002) How do we tell time? Neuroscientist 8:42–51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cassidy M, Mazzone P, Oliviero A, Insola A, Tonali P, Di Lazzaro V, Brown P (2002) Movement-related changes in synchronization in the human basal ganglia. Brain 125:1235–1246PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cassim F, Labyt E, Devos D, Defebvre L, Destée A, Derambure P (2002) Relationship between oscillations in the basal ganglia and synchronization of cortical activity. Epilepsy Dis 4:31–45Google Scholar
  12. Church RM, Deluty MZ (1977) Bisection of temporal intervals. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 3:216–228PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Duchek JM, Balota DA, Ferraro FR (1994) Component analysis of a rhythmic finger tapping task in individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type and in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 8:218–226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Elsinger CL, Rao SM, Zimbelman JL, Reynolds NC, Blindauer KA, Hoffman RG (2003) Neural basis for impaired time reproduction in Parkinson’s disease: an fMRI study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9:1088–1098PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) ‘Mini-mental state’: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Foltynie T, Brayne C, Barker RA (2002) The heterogeneity of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol 249:138–145PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibbon J (1977) Scalar expectancy theory, Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychol Rev 84:279–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gibbon J, Malapani C, Dale CL, Gallistel C (1997) Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:170–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Goldman-Rakic PS, Castner SA, Svensson TH, Siever LJ, Williams GV (2004) Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 174:3–16Google Scholar
  20. Grant DA, Berg EA (1948) A behavioral analysis of the degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card sorting problem. J Exp Psychol 38:404–411CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Harrington DL, Haaland KY (1999) Neural underpinnings of temporal processing: a review of focal lesion, pharmacological, and functional imaging research. Rev Neurosci 10:91–116PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Hermanowicz N (1998) Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology 12:3–12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hazeltine E, Helmuth LL, Ivry RB (1997) Neural mechanisms of timing. Trends Cogn Sci 1:163–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Heaton RK, Chelune GI, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G (1993) Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, OdessaGoogle Scholar
  25. Ivry RB, Hazeltine RE (1995) Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence of a common timing mechanism. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:3–18PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Ivry RB, Keele SW (1989) Timing functions of the cerebellum. J Cogn Neurosci 1:136–152Google Scholar
  27. Johnson RA, Wichern DW (1998) Applied multivariate statistical analysis. Prentice Hall, NJGoogle Scholar
  28. Keele S, Nicoletti R, Ivry R, Pokorny R (1985) Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correlational analysis. Acta Psychol 60:173–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Klockgether T, Borutta M, Rapp H, Spieker S, Dichgans J (1995) A defect of kinesthesia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 10:460–465PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lewis SJG, Foltynie T, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Owen AM, Barker RA (2005) Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease in the early clinical stages using a data driven approach. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76:343–348PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW (2004) Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd edn. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  32. Malapani C, Rakitin B, Levy R, Meck WH, Deweer B, Dubois B, Gibbon J (1998) Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson’s disease. A dopamine-related dysfunction. J Cogn Neurosci 10:316–331PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Maricq AV, Church RM (1983) The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 79:10–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Meck WH (1996) Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cogn Brain Res 3:227–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Meck WH (2006a) Frontal cortex lesions eliminate the clock speed effect of dopaminergic drugs on interval timing. Brain Res 1108:157–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Meck WH (2006b) Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: a functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems. Brain Res 1109:93–107PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Meck WH, Benson AM (2002) Dissecting the brain’s internal clock: how frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention. Brain Cogn 48:195–211PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Meck WH, Malapani C (2004) Neuroimaging of interval timing. Cogn Brain Res 21:133–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. O’Boyle DJ, Freeman JS, Cody FW (1996) The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 119:51–70PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pastor MA, Jahanshahi M, Artieda J, Obeso JA (1992a) Performance of repetitive wrist movements in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115:875–891PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pastor MA, Artieda J, Jahanshahi M, Obeso JA (1992b) Time estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115:211–225PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pekkonen E, Ahveninen J, Virtanen J, Teravainen H (1998) Parkinson’s disease selectively impairs preattentive auditory processing: an MEG study. Neuroreport 9:2949–2952PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Petrides M (1997) Visuo-motor conditional associative learning after frontal and temporal lesions in the human brain. Neuropsychologia 35:989–997PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Pouthas V, George N, Poline JB, Pfeuty M, VandeMoorteele PF, Hugueville L, Ferrandez AM, Lehericy S, LeBihan D, Renault B (2005) Neural network involoved in time perception: an fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 25:433–441PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Psychological Corp (1999) Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence manual. Psychological Corp, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  46. Rammsayer TH (1997) Are there dissociable roles of the mesostriatal and mesolimbocortical dopamine systems on temporal information processing in humans? Neuropsychobiology 35:36–45PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rao SM, Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Bobholz JA, Cox RW, Binder JR (1997) Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. J Neurosci 17:5528–5535PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Rao SM, Mayer AR, Harrington DL (2001) The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nat Neurosci 4:317–323PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schrag A, Quinn NP, Ben-Shlomo Y (2006) Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77:275–276PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Schubotz RI, Friederici AD, von Cramon YD (2000) Time perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 11:1–12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schultz W (2006) Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annu Rev Psychol 57:87–115PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Snedecor GW, Cochran WG (1989) Statistical methods. The Iowa State University Press, AmesGoogle Scholar
  53. Spencer RM, Ivry RB (2005) Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing. Brain Cogn 58:84–93PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN (2003) Weber (slope) analyses of timing variability in tapping and drawing tasks. J Mot Behav 35:371–381PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Treisman M, Faulkner A, Naish PL (1992) On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Q J Exp Psychol A 45:235–263PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Wearden JH (2004) Decision processes in models of timing. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 64:303–317Google Scholar
  57. Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale, 3rd edn. Psychological Corp, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  58. Wing AM, Kristofferson AB (1973) Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Percept Psychophys 14:5–12Google Scholar
  59. Zar JH (1996) Biostatistical analysis. Prentice Hall, NJGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Hugo Merchant
    • 1
  • Monica Luciana
    • 3
  • Catalina Hooper
    • 3
  • Stacy Majestic
    • 2
  • Paul Tuite
    • 2
  1. 1.Instituto de NeurobiologiaUNAMMexicoUSA
  2. 2.Department of NeurologyUniversity of MinnesotaMinneapolisUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MinnesotaMinneapolisUSA

Personalised recommendations