Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 183, Issue 3, pp 371–376 | Cite as

A repetition suppression effect lasting several days within the semantic network

  • Ingo G. Meister
  • Dorothee Buelte
  • Roland Sparing
  • Babak Boroojerdi
Research Article

Abstract

Performance in a semantic task is speeded up for repeated stimuli compared to novel stimuli. This conceptual priming effect is related to a decrease in functional activation within the left inferior prefrontal cortex for repeated stimulus exposure (repetition suppression). However, in contrast to perceptual priming which is known to be very robust over long periods of time, previous studies on semantic priming focused on short-term effects. The present study combined a behavioral and functional imaging experiment to investigate long-term conceptual repetition priming (retention interval 3 days). We found a highly significant decrease of reaction time for word stimuli which were presented repeatedly after 3 days both compared to initial presentation and to a matched word list. The functional magnetic resonance imaging data showed a repetition suppression within the left inferior (BA45, BA47) and middle (BA9) frontal gyrus for the comparison of known with unknown words. These data demonstrate that even over a period as long as 3 days, a repetition suppression within the left frontal network involved in semantic decision can be found. Thus, priming-related mechanisms in the semantic network may be robust over several days.

Keywords

Functional imaging Priming Semantic Psychophysical Implicit memory fMRI 

References

  1. Badre D, Wagner AD (2004) Selection, integration, and conflict monitoring; assessing the nature and generality of prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Neuron 41:473–487PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Becker S, Moscovitch M, Behrmann M, Joordens S (1997) Long-term semantic priming: a computational account and empirical evidence. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:1059–1082PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Binder JR, McKiernan KA, Parsons ME, Westbury CF, Possing ET, Kaufman JN, Buchanan L (2003) Neural correlates of lexical access during visual word recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 15:372–393PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Binder JR, Westbury CF, McKiernan KA, Possing ET, Medler DA (2005) Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. J Cogn Neurosci 17:905–917PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blasi V, Young AC, Tansy AP, Petersen SE, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M (2002) Word retrieval learning modulates right frontal cortex in patients with left frontal damage. Neuron 36:159–170PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bokde AL, Tagamets MA, Friedman RB, Horwitz B (2001) Functional interactions of the inferior frontal cortex during the processing of words and word-like stimuli. Neuron 30:609–617PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Buckner RL, Koustaal W, Schacter DL, Rosen BR (2000) Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming. Brain 123:620–640PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cave CB (1997) Very long-lasting priming in picture naming. Psychol Sci 8:322–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Copland DA, de Zubicaray GI, McMahon K, Wilson SJ, Eastburn M, Chenery HJ (2003) Brain activity during automatic semantic priming revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 20:302–310PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Damasio H, Tranel D, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, Damasio A (2004) Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition 92:179–229PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Demb JB, Desmond JE, Wagner AD, Vaidya CJ, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1995) Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity. J Neurosci 15:5870–5878PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dobbins IG, Schnyer DM, Verfaellie M, Schacter DL (2004) Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature 428:316–319PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Donaldson DI, Petersen SE, Buckner RL (2001) Dissociating memory retrieval processes using fMRI: evidence that priming does not support recognition memory. Neuron 31:1047–1059PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Friedrich FJ, Henik A, Tzelgov J (1991) Automatic processes in lexical access and spreading activation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:792–806PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Price CJ, Buchel C, Worsley KJ (1999) Multisubject fMRI studies and conjunction analyses. Neuroimage 10:385–396PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gardiner JM, Java RI (1991) Forgetting in recognition memory with and without recollective experience. Mem Cognit 19:617–623PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Goshen-Gottstein Y, Kempinsky H (2001) Probing memory with conceptual cues at multiple retention intervals: a comparison of forgetting rates on implicit and explicit tests. Psychon Bull Rev 8:139–146PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A (2006) Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn Sci 10:14–23PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hager W, Hasselhorn M (1994) Handbuch deutschsprachiger Wortnormen. Hogrefe, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  20. Henson RN (2003) Neuroimaging studies of priming. Prog Neurobiol 70:53–81PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Joordens S, Becker S (1997) The long and short of semantic priming effects in lexical decision. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:1083–1105PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kolers PA (1976) Pattern-analyzing memory. Science 191:1280–1281PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Maccotta L, Buckner RL (2004) Evidence for neural effects of repetition that directly correlate with behavioral priming. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1625–1632PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McDermott KB, Petersen SE, Watson JM, Ojemann JG (2003) A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41:293–303PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Meister IG, Weidemann J, Foltys H, Brand H, Willmes K, Krings T, Thron A, Topper R, Boroojerdi B (2005) The neural correlate of very-long-term picture priming. Eur J Neurosci 21:1101–1116PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mitchell DB, Brown AS (1988) Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its dissociation from recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:213–222PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Rissman J, Eliassen JC, Blumstein SE (2003) An event-related FMRI investigation of implicit semantic priming. J Cogn Neurosci 15:1160–1175PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Roediger HL, McDermott KB (1993) Implicit memory on normal human subjects. In: Boller F, Grafman J (Eds) Handbook of neuropsychology, vol 8. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 63–161Google Scholar
  29. Schnyer DM, Dobbins IG, Nicholls L, Schacter DL, Verfaellie M (2006) Rapid response learning in amnesia: delineating associative learning components in repetition priming. Neuropsychologia 44:140–149PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Schott BH, Henson RN, Richardson-Klavehn A, Becker C, Thoma V, Heinze HJ, Duzel E (2005) Redefining implicit and explicit memory: the functional neuroanatomy of priming, remembering, and control of retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:1257–1262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Schott BH, Richardson-Klavehn A, Henson RN, Becker C, Heinze HJ, Duzel E (2006) Neuroanatomical dissociation of encoding processes related to priming and explicit memory. J Neurosci 26:792–800PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sloman SA, Hayman CAG, Ohta N, Law J, Tulving E (1988) Forgetting in primed fragment completion. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:223–239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Thompson-Schill SL, Gabrieli JD (1999) Priming of visual and functional knowledge on a semantic classification task. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 25:41–53PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tulving E, Schacter DL (1990) Priming and human memory systems. Science 247:301-306PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tunney RJ (2003) Implicit and explicit knowledge decay at different rates: a dissociation between priming and recognition in artificial grammar learning. Exp Psychol 50:124–130PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JD, Keane MM, Monti LA Gutierrez-Rivas H, Zarella MM (1997) Evidence for multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory tests. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:1324–1343PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. van Turennout M, Ellmore T, Martin A (2000) Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system. Nat Neurosci 3:1329–1334PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Herve PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houde O, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (2006) Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 30:1414–1432PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wagner AD, Desmond JD, Demb JB, Glover GH, Gabrieli JDE (1997) Semantic repetition priming for verbal and pictorial knowldege: a functional MRI study of left inferior prefrontal cortex. J Cog Neurosci 9:714–724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wagner AD, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Schacter DL, Buckner RL (2000) Task-specific repetition priming in left inferior prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10:1176–1184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wig GS, Grafton ST, Demos KE, Kelley WM (2005) Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming. Nat Neurosci 8:1228–1233PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ingo G. Meister
    • 1
  • Dorothee Buelte
    • 2
  • Roland Sparing
    • 3
  • Babak Boroojerdi
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of NeurologyUniversity Hospital CologneCologneGermany
  2. 2.Department of NeurologyRWTH Aachen UniversityAachenGermany
  3. 3.Research Center JuelichInstitute of Neuroscience and BiophysicsJuelichGermany

Personalised recommendations