Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 179, Issue 4, pp 709–721 | Cite as

Effects of visual stimuli on temporal order judgments of unimanual finger stimuli

  • Satoshi Shibuya
  • Toshimitsu Takahashi
  • Shigeru Kitazawa
Research Article

Abstract

Successive tactile stimuli, delivered one to each hand, are referred to spatial representation before they are ordered in time (Yamamoto and Kitazawa in Nat Neurosci 4:759–765 2001a). In the present study, we examined if this applies even when they are delivered unilaterally to fingers of a single hand. Tactile stimuli were delivered left-to-rightward relative to the body (2nd–3rd–4th) or in reverse with stimulus onset asynchrony of 100 ms. Simultaneously with the delivery of tactile stimuli, three of nine small squares arranged in a matrix of 3 × 3 were turned on as if they appeared near the tips of the fingers. Although subjects were instructed to ignore the visual stimuli and make a forced choice between the two orders of tactile stimuli, the correct-judgment probability depended on the direction of visual stimuli. It was greater than 95% when the direction of visual stimuli matched that of the tactile stimuli, but less than 50% when they were opposite to each other. When the right hand was rotated counterclockwise on the horizontal plane (90°) so that the fingers were pointing to the left, the preferred direction of visual stimuli that yielded the peak correct judgment was also rotated, although not to the full extent. These results show that subjects cannot be basing their tactile temporal order judgment solely on a somatotopic map, but rather on a spatial map on which both visual and tactile signals converge.

Keywords

Tactile temporal order judgment Multisensory integration Hand posture Apparent motion Frames of reference 

Notes

Acknowledgment

We thank Dr. T. Uka for comments on the manuscript. The study was partly supported by the Human Frontier Science Program and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (A) #18200024 to S.K.

References

  1. Armel KC, Ramachandran VS (2003) Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc Biol Sci 270:1499–1506PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker CL Jr, Hess RF, Zihl J (1991) Residual motion perception in a “motion-blind” patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli. J Neurosci 11:454–461PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Craig JC (2006) Visual motion interferes with tactile motion perception. Perception 35:351–367PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gallace A, Spence C (2005) Visual capture of apparent limb position influences tactile temporal order judgments. Neurosci Lett 379:63–68PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Graziano MS (1999) Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:10418–10421PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Graziano MS, Cooke DF (2006) Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia 44:845–859PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Graziano MS, Cooke DF, Taylor CS (2000) Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science 290:1782–1786PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hagen MC, Franzen O, McGlone F, Essick G, Dancer C, Pardo JV (2002) Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex. Eur J Neurosci 16:957–964PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hirsh IJ, Sherrick CE Jr (1961) Perceived order in different sense modalities. J Exp Psychol 62:423–432PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Holmes NP, Spence C (2004) The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cogn Process 5:94–105PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Howard IP, Templeton WB (1966) Human spatial orientation. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport 7:2325–2330PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Iriki A, Tanaka M, Obayashi S, Iwamura Y (2001) Self-images in the video monitor coded by monkey intraparietal neurons. Neurosci Res 40:163–173PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kirman JH (1974) Tactile apparent movement: the effects of number of stimulators. J Exp Psychol 103:1175–1180PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kirman JH (1983) Tactile apparent movement: the effects of shape and type of motion. Percept Psychophys 34:96–102PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:244–247PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Libet B (2006) Reflections on the interaction of the mind and brain. Prog Neurobiol 78:322–326PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lloyd DM, Shore DI, Spence C, Calvert GA (2003) Multisensory representation of limb position in human premotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 6:17–18PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lyons G, Sanabria D, Vatakis A, Spence C (2006) The modulation of crossmodal integration by unimodal perceptual grouping: a visuotactile apparent motion study. Exp Brain Res 174:510–516Google Scholar
  21. Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J (2003) Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach. Curr Biol 13: R531–R539PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mikami A, Newsome WT, Wurtz RH (1986a) Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. I. Mechanisms of direction and speed selectivity in extrastriate area MT. J Neurophysiol 55:1308–1327Google Scholar
  23. Mikami A, Newsome WT, Wurtz RH (1986b) Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. II. Spatiotemporal range of directional interactions in MT and V1. J Neurophysiol 55:1328–1339Google Scholar
  24. Newsome WT, Pare EB (1988) A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J Neurosci 8:2201–2211PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Obayashi S, Tanaka M, Iriki A (2000) Subjective image of invisible hand coded by monkey intraparietal neurons. Neuroreport 11:3499–3505Google Scholar
  26. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J (2000) Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychol Sci 11:353–359PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pöppel E (1997) A hierarchical model of temporal perception. . Trends Cogn Sci 1:56–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ricciardi E, Vanello N, Dente D, Scilingo EP, Gentili C, Sani L, Haxby JV, Landini L, Bicchi A, Pietrini P (2004) Optic and tactile flow activate V5/MT in the human brain. 2004 Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner. San Diego, CA: Society for Neuroscience, 2004. Online. 643.14Google Scholar
  30. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1997) The space around us. Science 277:190–191PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Roder B, Rosler F, Spence C (2004) Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind. Curr Biol 14:121–124PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Sanabria D, Soto-Faraco S, Spence C (2005a) Assessing the effect of visual and tactile distractors on the perception of auditory apparent motion. Exp Brain Res 166:548–558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sanabria D, Soto-Faraco S, Spence C (2005b) Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture. Exp. Brain Res 165:505–514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sherrick CE (1976) The antagonisms of hearing and touch. In: Hearing and Davis: Essays Honoring Hallowell Davis. Washington University Press, St. Louis, MO, pp 149–158Google Scholar
  35. Shore DI, Gray K, Spry E, Spence C (2005) Spatial modulation of tactile temporal-order judgments. Perception 34:1251–1262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14:153–163PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Soto-Faraco S, Kingstone A (2004) Multisensory integration of dynamic information. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) The handbook of multisensory processes. The MIT press, London, pp 49–67Google Scholar
  38. Soto-Faraco S, Lyons J, Gazzaniga M, Spence C, Kingstone A (2002) The ventriloquist in motion: illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities. Cogn Brain Res 14:139–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2004a) Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: extending the phenomenon of cross-modal dynamic capture. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:208–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2004b) Cross-modal dynamic capture: congruency effects in the perception of motion across sensory modalities. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:330–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2005) Assessing automaticity in the audiovisual integration of motion. Acta Psychol (Amst) 118:71–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Spence C, Pavani F, Driver J (2004) Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:148–169PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spence C, Shore DI, Klein RM (2001) Multisensory prior entry. J Exp Psychol Gen 130:799–832PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sternberg S, Knoll R (1973) The perception of temporal order: fundamental issues and a general model. In: Kornblum S (ed) Attention and performance. Academic, New York, pp 629–685Google Scholar
  45. Takahashi T, Shibuya S, Kowatari Y, Yamamoto M, Kitazawa S (2004) Neural correlates of temporal order judgment - an fMRI study. 2004 Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner. San Diego, CA: Society for Neuroscience, 2004. Online. 321.2Google Scholar
  46. Wada M, Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2004) Effects of handedness on tactile temporal order judgment. Neuropsychologia 42:1887–1895PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wann JP, Ibrahim SF (1992) Does limb proprioception drift? Exp Brain Res 91:162–166PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Woods TM, Recanzone GH (2004) Cross-modal interactions evidenced by the ventriloquism effect in human and monkeys. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) The handbook of multisensory processes. The MIT Press, London, pp 35–48Google Scholar
  49. Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001a) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4:759–765CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001b) Sensation at the tips of invisible tools. Nat Neurosci 4:979–980CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Yamamoto S, Moizumi S, Kitazawa S (2005) Referral of tactile sensation to the tips of L-shaped sticks. J Neurophysiol 93:2856–2863PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Zampini M, Harris C, Spence C (2005) Effect of posture change on tactile perception: impaired direction discrimination performance with interleaved fingers. Exp Brain Res 166:498–508PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Satoshi Shibuya
    • 1
  • Toshimitsu Takahashi
    • 2
    • 3
  • Shigeru Kitazawa
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Integrative PhysiologyKyorin University School of MedicineTokyoJapan
  2. 2.Department of NeurophysiologyJuntendo University Graduate School of MedicineTokyoJapan
  3. 3.CREST, Japan Science and Technology AgencySaitamaJapan

Personalised recommendations