Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 175, Issue 4, pp 710–725 | Cite as

Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: switching between cyclical and discrete units of action

  • John J. Buchanan
  • Jin-Hoon Park
  • Charles H. Shea
Research Article

Abstract

An aiming task was used to identify the processes whereby the motor system adapted a repetitive aiming action to systematic changes in ID (ID = log2 (2A/W), Fitts in J Exp Psychol 47:381–391, 1954) within a single trial. Task ID was scaled in a trial by moving the outside edge of two stationary targets to produce nine different target IDs in a trail. The ID within a trial was scaled in one of two directions: (1) an increasing ID condition, starting with an ID = 3.07 and ending with an ID = 5.91; and (2) a decreasing ID condition, starting with an ID = 5.91 and ending with an ID = 3.07. An index of movement harmonicity (Guiard in Acta Psychol 82:139–159, 1993) revealed that the repetitive aiming action was harmonic in nature when task ID was 3.07, and consisted of a series of discrete segments when task ID was 5.91. This finding provides evidence for the existence of discrete and cyclical units of action that are irreducible and that may be employed independently to assemble longer continuous actions. The scaling of ID within a trial promoted a transition in repetitive aiming motions assembled from discrete and cyclical units of action. A variety of kinematic measures (e.g., movement harmonicity, time spent accelerating the limb) revealed a critical ID (IDc) region (4.01–4.91) separating aiming motions governed by the different units of action. Enhancement of fluctuations before the transition were found in the movement harmonicity data and in the distance traveled to peak velocity data, with variability in these measures highest in the IDc region. The enhancement of fluctuations indicates that loss of stability in the limb’s motion acted as a key mechanism underlying the transition between units of action. The loss of stability was associated with the transition from cyclical to discrete actions and with the transition from discrete to cyclical actions. The transition between units of action may be conceptualized as a transition from a limit cycle attractor (cyclical unit of action) to a shift between two fixed-point attractors (discrete unit of action) when ID was increased, with the transition occurring in the opposite direction when ID was decreased.

Keywords

Fitts’ law Limit cycle Fixed point Loss of stability Critical boundary Planning and feedback 

References

  1. Abend W, Bizzi E, Morasso P (1982) Human arm trajectory formation. Brain 105:331–348PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Adam JJ, Paas FGWC (1996) Dwell time in reciprocal aiming tasks. Hum Mov Sci 15:1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Adam JJ, van der Bruggen DPW, Bekkering H (1993) The control of discrete and reciprocal target-aiming responses: evidence for the exploitation of mechanics. Hum Mov Sci 12:353–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Adamovich SV, Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) Merging different motor patterns: coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 99:325–337PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Asatryan DG, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of steady posture, I. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the joint on execution of a postural task. Biophysics 10:925–934Google Scholar
  6. Athenes S, Sallagoity I, Zanone PG, Albaret JM (2004) Evaluating the coordination dynamics of handwriting. Hum Mov Sci 23:621–641PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Billon M, Bootsma RJ, Mottet D (2000) The dynamics of human isometric pointing movements under varying accuracy requirements. Neurosci Lett 286:49–52PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS (1993) Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Exp Brain Res 94:131–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU (2005) The interaction of tactile information and movement amplitude in a multijoint bimanual circle-tracing task: phase transitions and loss of stability. Q J Exp Psychol 50A:769–787Google Scholar
  10. Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, deGuzman GD (1997a) Self-organization of trajectory information. Biol Cybern 76:257–273PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, deGuzman GD, Ding M (1997b) The spontaneous recruitment and suppression of degrees of freedom in rhythmic hand movements. Hum Mov Sci 16:1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buchanan JJ, Park J-H, Ryu YU, Shea CH (2003) Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Exp Brain Res 150:473–489PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Buchanan JJ, Park J-H, Shea CH (2004) Systematic scaling of target width: dynamics, planning, and feedback. Neurosci Lett 367:317–322PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Byblow WD, Carson RG, Goodman D (1994) Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Hum Mov Sci 13:3–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carson RG, Goodman D, Kelso JAS, Elliott D (1995) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in rhythmic coordination of ipsilateral hand and foot. J Mot Behav 27:211–224PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cohen J (1971) Synchronous bimanual movements performed by homologous and non-homologous muscles. Percept Mot Skills 32:639–644PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Crossman ERFW, Goodeve PJ (1983) Feedback control of hand-movement and Fitts’ law. Q J Exp Psychol 35A:251–278Google Scholar
  18. Cullen JD, Helsen WF, Buekers MJ, Hesketh KL, Starkes JL, Elliott D (2001) The utilization of visual information in the control of reciprocal aiming movements. Hum Mov Sci 20:807–828PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. deGuzman GD, Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ (1997) Self-organization of trajectory formation. Biol Cybern 76:275–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting error in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium point hypothesis (λ-model) for motor control. J Mot Behav 18:17–54PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grafton ST, Mazziotta JC, Woods RP, Phelps ME (1992) Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115:565–587PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Guiard Y (1993) On Fitts’s and Hooke’s laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychol 82:139–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Guiard Y (1997) Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Hum Mov Sci 16:97–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haken H (1983) Synergetics. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  27. Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Haken H, Peper CE, Beek PJ, Daffertshofer A (1996) A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping. Physica D 90:179–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Heath M, Hodges NJ, Chua R, Elliott D (1998) On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. Can J Exp Psychol 52:163–173Google Scholar
  30. Hollerbach JM (1981) An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biol Cybern 39:139–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS (2004) The excitator as a minimal model for the coordination dynamics of discrete and rhythmic movement generation. J Mot Behav 37:35–51Google Scholar
  32. Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol [Reg Integ Comp] 15:R1000–R1004Google Scholar
  33. Kelso JAS (1992) Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skill: from information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. J Exp Psychol Gen 121:260–262PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  35. Kelso JAS, Jeka JJ (1992) Symmetry breaking dynamics of human multilimb coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:645–668PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kelso JAS, Scholz JAS, Schöner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Wallace SA (1991) Order parameters for the neural organization of single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Exp Brain Res 85:432–445PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, deGuzman GC, Ding M (1993). Spontaneous recruitment and annihilation of degrees of freedom in biological coordination. Phys Lett A 179:364–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Khan MA, Franks IM (2000) The effect of practice on component submovements is dependent on the availability of visual feedback. J Mot Behav 32:227–240PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Khan MA, Franks IM (2003) Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 35:285–295PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Khan MA, Franks IM, Goodman D (1998) The effect of practice on the control of rapid aiming movements: evidence for an interdependency between programming and feedback processing. Q J Exp Psychol 51A:425–444Google Scholar
  42. Krebs HI, Aisen ML, Volpe BT, Hogan N (1999) Quantization of continuous arm movements in humans with brain injury. Neurobiology 96:4645–4649Google Scholar
  43. Meyer DE, Kornblum S, Abrams RA, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 42:223–227PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mottet D, Bootsma RJ (1999) The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming. Biol Cybern 80:235–245PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mottet D, Guiard Y, Ferrand T, Bootsma RJ (2001) Two-handed performance of a rhythmical Fitts’ task by individuals and dyads. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:1275–1286PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mourik AM, Beek PJ (2004) Discrete and cyclical movements: unified dynamics or separate control. Acta Psychol 117:121–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Peper CE, Beek PJ, van Wieringen PCW (1995) Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1117–1138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Plamondon R, Alimi AM (1997) Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behav Brain Sci 20:279–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. de Rugy A, Sternad D (2003) Interaction between discrete and rhythmic movements: reaction time and phase of discrete movement initiation during oscillatory movements. Brain Res 994:160–174PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ryu YU, Buchanan JJ (2004) Amplitude scaling in a bimanual circle drawing task: pattern switching and end-effector variability. J Mot Behav 36:265–275PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Schaal S, Sternad D, Osu R, Kawato M (2004) Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete. Nat Neurosci 7:1137–1144Google Scholar
  53. Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:227–247PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schöner G (1990) A dynamic theory of coordination of discrete movement. Biol Cybern 63:257–270PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schöner G (1994) From interlimb coordination to trajectory formation: common dynamical principles. In: Swinnen SP, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P (eds) Interlimb coordination: neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints. Academic, Boston, pp 339–368Google Scholar
  56. Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239:1513–1520PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS (1986) A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biol Cybern 53:247–257PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Soechting JF, Terzuolo CA (1987) Organization of arm movements in three-dimensional space. Wrist motion is piecewise planar. Neuroscience 23:53–61PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sternad D, Dean WJ (2003) Rhythmic and discrete elements in multi-joint coordination. Brain Res 989:152–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sternad D, Schaal S (1999) Segmentation of endpoint trajectories does not imply segmented control. Exp Brain Res 124:118–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sternad D, Dean WJ, Schaal S (2000) Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements. Hum Mov Sci 19:627–664CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Thoroughman KA, Shadmehr R (2000) Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature 407:742–747PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Turvey MT (1990) Coordination. Am Psychol 45:938–953PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Winstein CJ, Pohl PS (1995) Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed hand movements. Exp Brain Res 105:163–174PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Winstein CJ, Grafton ST, Pohl PS (1997) Motor task difficulty and brain activity: investigation of goal-directed aiming using positron emission tomography. J Neurophysiol 77:1581–1594PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • John J. Buchanan
    • 1
  • Jin-Hoon Park
    • 2
  • Charles H. Shea
    • 1
  1. 1.Human Performance Laboratories, Department of Health and KinesiologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationUSA
  2. 2.Department of Physical EducationKorea UniversitySeoulKorea

Personalised recommendations