Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 174, Issue 3, pp 499–509 | Cite as

Serial correlation in lateralized choices of hand and target

Research Article

Abstract

We investigated how lateralized choices of hand and target are influenced by previous behavior. Three monkeys retrieved food pellets following cues indicating the location of available food pellet targets, and the hand that could be used to acquire a target. In pseudo-randomized trials, the monkeys could retrieve food pellet targets only on their right side, only on their left side, or their choice of either right or left side, using only their right hand, only their left hand, or their choice of either hand. We examined separately the patterns of serial correlation in target choices and hand choices. Although individual monkeys showed overall laterality preferences, instead of repeatedly using the preferred hand, we found that the monkeys tended to switch hands in successive trials. This serial correlation in hand choice was stronger and more robust than serial correlation in target choice. Furthermore, the pattern of serial correlation for target choice closely resembled that of serial correlation for hand choice when the animal was allowed to choose both target and hand, but only when the target cue was presented before the hand cue. These results suggest that when cued to choose a hand first, the monkeys tended to make a separate decision as to whether to switch their target choices or not, whereas their decisions to switch hands and targets were linked more tightly if the animal was cued to choose a target first.

Keywords

Decision making Monkeys Preference Premotor cortex Selection Strategy Switching 

References

  1. Barraclough DJ, Conroy ML, Lee D (2004) Prefrontal cortex and decision making in a mixed-strategy game. Nat Neurosci 7:404–410PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Byrne RW, Whiten A (1988) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  3. Christensen R (1997) Log-linear models and logistic regression. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  4. Deuel RK, Dunlop NL (1980) Hand preferences in the rhesus monkey. Implications for the study of cerebral dominance. Arch Neurol 37:217–221PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Gardinier J, Franco V, Schieber MH (2006) Interactions between lateralized choices of hand and target. Exp Brain Res 170:149–159PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Genovesio A, Brasted PJ, Mitz AR, Wise SP (2005) Prefrontal cortex activity related to abstract response strategies. Neuron 47:307–320PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Herrnstein RJ, Rachlin H, Laibson DI (1997) The matching law: papers in psychology and economics. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  8. Hopkins WD, Cantalupo C (2005) Individual and setting differences in the hand preferences of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a critical analysis and some alternative explanations. Laterality 10:65–80PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Hopkins WD, Washburn DA, Rumbaugh DM (1989) Note on hand use in the manipulation of joysticks by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 103:91–94PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hoshi E, Tanji J (2000) Integration of target and body-part information in the premotor cortex when planning action. Nature 408:466–470PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hoshi E, Tanji J (2004a) Area-selective neuronal activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for information retrieval and action planning. J Neurophysiol 91:2707–2722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hoshi E, Tanji J (2004b) Differential roles of neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas: from information retrieval to motor planning and execution. J Neurophysiol 92:3482–3499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lee D (2006) Neural basis of quasi-rational decision making. Curr Opin Neurobiol (in press)Google Scholar
  14. Lee D, Conroy ML, McGreevy BP, Barraclough DJ (2004) Reinforcement learning and decision making in monkeys during a competitive game. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22:45–58PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lee D, McGreevy BP, Barraclough DJ (2005) Learning and decision making in monkeys during a rock-paper-scissors game. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25:416–430PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lehman RA (1980) Distribution and changes in strength of hand preference of cynomolgus monkeys. Brain Behav Evol 17:209–217PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lewis DA, Melchitzky DS, Sesack SR, Whitehead RE, Auh S, Sampson A (2001) Dopamine transporter immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex: regional, laminar, and ultrastructural localization. J Comp Neurol 432:119–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. MacNeilage PF, Studdert-Kennedy MG, Lindblom B (1987) Primate handedness reconsidered. Behav Brain Sci 10:247–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Matsuzaka Y, Tanji J (1996) Changing directions of forthcoming arm movements: neuronal activity in the presupplementary and supplementary motor area of monkey cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol 76:2327–2342PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Mussa Ivaldi FA, Morasso P, Zaccaria R (1988) Kinematic networks. A distributed model for representing and regularizing motor redundancy. Biol Cybern 60:1–16PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. von Neumann J, Morgenstern O (1944) Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  22. Rigamonti MM, Previde EP, Poli MD, Marchant LF, McGrew WC (1998) Methodology of motor skill and laterality: new test of hand preference in Macaca nemestrina. Cortex 34:693–705PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Schieber MH (2000) Inactivation of the ventral premotor cortex biases the laterality of motoric choices. Exp Brain Res 130:497–507PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Schultz W (1998) Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 80:1–27PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Shima K, Mushiake H, Saito N, Tanji J (1996) Role for cells in the presupplementary motor area in updating motor plans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:8694–8698PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Soltani A, Lee D, Wang X-J (2006) Neural mechanism for stochastic behavior during a competitive game. Neural Netw (in press)Google Scholar
  27. Spinozzi G, Cacchiarelli B (2000) Manual laterality in haptic and visual reaching tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). An association between hand preference and hand accuracy for food discrimination. Neuropsychologia 38:1685–1692PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  29. Sutton RS, Barto AG (1998) Reinforcement learning: an introduction. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  30. Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5:1226–1235PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Vauclair J, Meguerditchian A, Hopkins WD (2005) Hand preferences for unimanual and coordinated bimanual tasks in baboons (Papio anubis). Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25:210–216PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Westergaard GC, Lussier ID, Higley JD (2001) Between-species variation in the development of hand preference among macaques. Neuropsychologia 39:1373–1378PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Center for Visual ScienceUniversity of RochesterRochesterUSA
  2. 2.Department of NeurologyUniversity of RochesterRochesterUSA
  3. 3.Department of Neurobiology and AnatomyUniversity of RochesterRochesterUSA

Personalised recommendations