Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 173, Issue 4, pp 612–622 | Cite as

The subjective visual vertical and the perceptual upright

  • Richard T. Dyde
  • Michael R. Jenkin
  • Laurence R. Harris
Research Article

Abstract

The direction of ‘up’ has traditionally been measured by setting a line (luminous if necessary) to the apparent vertical, a direction known as the ‘subjective visual vertical’ (SVV); however for optimum performance in visual skills including reading and facial recognition, an object must to be seen the ‘right way up’—a separate direction which we have called the ‘perceptual upright’ (PU). In order to measure the PU, we exploited the fact that some symbols rely upon their orientation for recognition. Observers indicated whether the symbol ‘ Open image in new window ’ presented in various orientations was identified as either the letter ‘p’ or the letter ‘d’. The average of the transitions between ‘p-to-d’ and ‘d-to-p’ interpretations was taken as the PU. We have labelled this new experimental technique the Oriented CHAracter Recognition Test (OCHART). The SVV was measured by estimating whether a line was rotated clockwise or counter-clockwise relative to gravity. We measured the PU and SVV while manipulating the orientation of the visual background in different observer postures: upright, right side down and (for the PU) supine. When the body, gravity and the visual background were aligned, the SVV and the PU were similar, but as the background orientation and observer posture orientations diverged, the two measures varied markedly. The SVV was closely aligned with the direction of gravity whereas the PU was closely aligned with the body axis. Both probes showed influences of all three cues (body orientation, vision and gravity) and these influences could be predicted from a weighted vectorial sum of the directions indicated by these cues. For the SVV, the ratio was 0.2:0.1:1.0 for the body, visual and gravity cues, respectively. For the PU, the ratio was 2.6:1.2:1.0. In the case of the PU, these same weighting values were also predicted by a measure of the reliability of each cue; however, reliability did not predict the weightings for the SVV. This is the first time that maximum likelihood estimation has been demonstrated in combining information between different reference frames. The OCHART technique provides a new, simple and readily applicable method for investigating the PU which complements the SVV. Our findings suggest that OCHART is particularly suitable for investigating the functioning of visual and non-visual systems and their contributions to the perceived upright of novel environments such as high- and low-g environments, and in patient and ageing populations, as well as for normal observers.

Keywords

Gravity perception Multi-sensory integration Vestibular Visual perception Orientation Perceived vertical 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Supported by NASA Cooperative Agreement NCC9-58 with the National Space Biomedical Research Institute, the Canadian Space Agency, and grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to L.R. Harris and M.R. Jenkin. We would like to thank Ian Howard, Carolee Orme and Chuck Oman for their comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

References

  1. Adams WJ, Graf EW, Ernst MO (2004) Experience can change the ‘light-from-above’ prior Nat Neurosci 7:1057–1058PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Asch SE, Witkin HA (1948) Studies in space perception. II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted J Exp Psychol 38:455–477CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Aubert H (1861) Eine scheinbare Drehung von Objekten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links Virchows Archiven 20:381–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bischof N (1974) Optic-vestibular orientation to the vertical In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology Springer Berlin, Heidelberg, New York pp 155–190Google Scholar
  5. Bockisch CJ, Haslwanter T (2001) Three-dimensional eye position during static roll and pitch in humans Vision Res 41:2127–2137PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bronstein AM, Perennou DA, Guerraz M, Playford D, Rudge P (2003) Dissociation of visual and haptic vertical in two patients with vestibular nuclear lesions Neurology 61:1260–1262PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Corballis MC, Zbrodoff NJ, Shetzer LI, Butler PB (1978) Decisions about identity and orientation of rotated letters and digits Mem Cognit 6:98–107PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Dyde RT, Sadr S, Jenkin MR, Jenkin HL, Harris LR (2004) The perceived direction of “up” measured using a p/d letter probe J Vis 4:385aCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Edelman S, Bulthoff HH (1992) Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of three-dimensional objects Vision Res 32:2385–2400PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion Nature 415:429–33PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Goodenough DR, Sigman E, Oltman PK, Rosso J, Mertz H (1979) Eye torsion in response to a tilted visual stimulus Vision Res 19:1177–1179PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Guerraz M, Poquin D, Ohlmann T (1998) The role of head-centric spatial reference with a static and kinetic visual disturbance Percept Psychophys 60:287–295PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Harris LR, Dyde RT, Sadr S, Jenkin MR, Jenkin HL (2004) Visual and vestibular contributions to the perceived direction of “up” J Vest Res 14:200–201Google Scholar
  14. Hillis JM, Ernst MO, Banks MS, Landy MS (2002) Combining sensory information: mandatory fusion within, but not between, senses Science 298:1627–1630PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Howard IP (1982) Human visual orientation Wiley New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. Howard IP, Childerson L (1994) The contribution of motion, the visual frame, and visual polarity to sensations of body tilt Perception 23:753–762PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Howard IP, Templeton WB (1964) Visually-induced eye torsion and tilt adaptation Vision Res 4:433–477PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Howard IP, Bergstrom SS, Ohmi M (1990) Shape from shading in different frames of reference Perception 19:523–530PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jenkin HL, Howard IP (1998) Retinal and gravitational frames of reference in ambiguous figure recognition Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 39:S858Google Scholar
  20. Jenkin HL, Jenkin M, Dyde RT, Harris LR (2004) Shape-from-shading depends on visual, gravitational, and body-orientation cues Perception 33:1453–1461PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jenkin HL, Dyde RT, Zacher JE, Zikovitz DC, Jenkin MR, Allison RS, Howard IP, Harris LR (2005) Relative role of visual and non-visual cues determining the direction of ‘up’: experiments in parabolic flight Acta Astronaut 56:1025–1032PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jolicoeur P (1985) The time to name disoriented natural objects Mem Cognit 13:289–303PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kaptein RG, Van Gisbergen JAM (2004) Interpretation of a discontinuity in the sense of verticality at large body tilt J Neurophysiol 91:2205–2214PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Maki RH (1986) Naming and locating the tops of rotated pictures Can J Psychol 40:368–387PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Mamassian P, Goutcher R (2001) Prior knowledge on the illumination position Cognition 81:B1–B9PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mamassian P, Kersten D (1996) Illumination, shading and the perception of local orientation Vision Res 36:2351–2367PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mars F, Vercher JL, Blouin J (2004) Perception of the vertical with a head-mounted visual frame during head tilt Ergonomics 47:1116–1130PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mast FW (2000) Does the world rock when the eyes roll? Swiss J Psychol 59:89–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McMullen PA, Jolicoeur P (1992) Reference frame and effects of orientation of finding the tops of rotated objects J Exp Psychol: Human Perc Perf 3:807–820CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Miller EF, Graybiel A (1971) Effect of gravitoinertial force on ocular counterrolling J Appl Physiol 31:697–700PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Miller EF, Graybiel A (1972) Human counterrolling measured during eight hours of sustained body tilt Minerva Ostorinolaringol 24:247–252Google Scholar
  32. Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action Oxford University Press OxfordGoogle Scholar
  33. Mittelstaedt H (1983) A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical Naturwissenschaften 70:272–281PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Mittelstaedt H (1986) The subjective vertical as a function of visual and extraretinal cues Acta Psychol 63:63–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mittelstaedt H (1988) The information processing structure of the subjective vertical. A cybernetic bridge between its psychophysics and its neurobiology In: Marko H, Hauske G, Struppler A (ed) Processing structures for perception and action VCH Weinheim pp 217–263Google Scholar
  36. Mittelstaedt H (1999) The role of the otoliths in perception of the vertical and in path integration Ann NY Acad Sci 871:334–344PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nyborg H (1980) Psychological differentiation in school children. Maturation, cognition and personality development. Psychological Reports Aarhus 5 University of Aarhus DenmarkGoogle Scholar
  38. Press WH (1988) Numerical recipes in C Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
  39. Ramachandran VS (1988) The perception of shape from shading Nature 331:163–166PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rock I (1973) Orientation and form Academic New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Rock I, Heimer W (1957) The effect of retinal and phenomenal orientation on the perception of form Am J Psychol 70:493–511PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rock I, Schreiber C, Ro T (1994) The dependence of two-dimensional shape perception on orientation Perception 23:1409–1426PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Thompson P (1980) Margaret Thatcher: a new illusion Perception 9:483–484PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Valentine T (1988) Upside-down faces: a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition Br J Psychol 79(Pt 4):471–491PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Van Beuzekom AD, Van Gisbergen JAM (2000) Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates J Neurophysiol 84: 11–27PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Wade SW, Curthoys IS (1997) The effect of ocular torsional position on perception of the roll-tilt of visual stimuli Vision Res 37:1071–1078PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wenderoth P, Hickey N (1993) Object and head orientation effects on symmetry perception defined by shape from shading Perception 22:1121–1130PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Wexler M, Lamouret I, Droulez J (2001a) The stationarity hypothesis: an allocentric criterion in visual perception Vision Res 41:3023–3037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wexler M, Panerai F, Lamouret I, Droulez J (2001b) Self-motion and the perception of stationary objects Nature 409:85–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Witkin HA (1949) Perception of body position and the position of the visual field Psychol Monog 63:1–63Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Richard T. Dyde
    • 1
  • Michael R. Jenkin
    • 1
    • 2
  • Laurence R. Harris
    • 1
    • 3
  1. 1.Centre for Vision ResearchYork UniversityTorontoCanada
  2. 2.Department of Computer Science and EngineeringYork UniversityTorontoCanada
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyYork UniversityTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations