Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 164, Issue 3, pp 301–310 | Cite as

Multiple frames of reference for pointing to a remembered target

  • Martin LemayEmail author
  • George E. Stelmach
Research Article

Abstract

Pointing with an unseen hand to a visual target that disappears prior to movement requires maintaining a memory representation about the target location. The target location can be transformed either into a hand-centered frame of reference during target presentation and remembered under that form, or remembered in terms of retinal and extra-retinal cues and transformed into a body-centered frame of reference before movement initiation. The main goal of the present study was to investigate whether the target is stored in memory in an eye-centered frame, a hand-centered frame or in both frames of reference concomitantly. The task was to locate, memorize, and point to a target in a dark environment. Hand movement was not visible. During the recall delay, participants were asked to move their hand or their eyes in order to disrupt the memory representation of the target. Movement of the eyes during the recall delay was expected to disrupt an eye-centered memory representation whereas movement of the hand was expected to disrupt a hand-centered memory representation by increasing movement variability to the target. Variability of movement amplitude and direction was examined. Results showed that participants were more variable on the directional component of the movement when required to move their hand during recall delay. On the contrary, moving the eyes caused an increase in variability only in the amplitude component of the pointing movement. Taken together, these results suggest that the direction of the movement is coded and remembered in a frame of reference linked to the arm, whereas the amplitude of the movement is remembered in an eye-centered frame of reference.

Keywords

Target Pointing Memory Eye Hand 

References

  1. Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J Neurosci 5:2318–2330PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley AD (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  3. Bennett PJ, Sekuler AB, McIntosh AR, Della-Maggiore V (2001) The effects of aging on visual memory: evidence for functional reorganization of cortical networks. Acta Psychol 107:249–73Google Scholar
  4. Berkinblit MB, Fookson OI, Smetanin B, Adamovich SV, Poizner H (1995) The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets. Exp Brain Res 107:326–330Google Scholar
  5. Bock O (1986) Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements. Exp Brain Res 64:476–482CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bock O (1993) Localization of objects in the peripheral visual field. Behav Brain Res 56:77–84Google Scholar
  7. Bock O, Arnold K (1993) Error accumulation and error correction in sequential pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 95:111–117Google Scholar
  8. Bridgeman B, Graziano JA (1989) Effect of context and efference copy on visual straight ahead. Vision Res 29:1729–1736Google Scholar
  9. Buneo CA, Jarvis MR, Batista AP, Andersen RA (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature 416:632–636CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Carrozzo M, McIntyre J, Zago M, Lacquaniti F (1999) Viewer-centered and body-centered frames of reference in direct visuomotor transformations. Exp Brain Res 129:201–210CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chieffi S, Allport DA, Woodin M (1999) Hand-centred coding of target location in visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 37:495–502Google Scholar
  12. Churchill A, Hopkins B, Rönnqvist L, Vogt S (2000) Vision of the hand and environmental context in human prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:81–89Google Scholar
  13. Coello Y, Magne P (2000) Determination of target distance in a structured environment: selection of visual information for action. Eur J Cogn Psychol 12:489–519Google Scholar
  14. Darling WG, Miller GF (1993) Transformation between visual and kinesthetic coordinate systems in reaches to remembered objects locations and orientations. Exp Brain Res 93:534–547Google Scholar
  15. Elliott D, Madalena J (1987) The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. Q J Exp Psychol 39A:541–559Google Scholar
  16. Enright JT (1995) The non-visual impact of eye orientation on eye-hand coordination. Vision Res 35:1611–1618CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Flanders M, Helms-Tillery SI, Soechting JF (1992) Early stages in sensorimotor transformation. Behav Brain Sci 15:309–362Google Scholar
  18. Georgopoulos AP (1997) Motor cortex: neural and computational studies. In: Donahoe JW, Packard Dorsel V (eds) Neural networks models of cognition. Elsevier, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  19. Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movement I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Exp Brain Res 99:97–111PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Graaf JB de, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ (1994) Misdirections in slow, goal-directed movements are not primarily visually-based. Exp Brain Res 99:464–472Google Scholar
  21. Grealy MA, Coello Y, Heffernan D (2003) Apparent motion cues distort object localization in egocentric space. Exp Brain Res 150:356–362Google Scholar
  22. Guay M (1986) Proactive interference and intertrial interval in short-term retention of temporal visual information. Percept Mot Skills 63:839–846Google Scholar
  23. Guay M, Hall CR (1984) Rehearsal of temporal visual information. Percept Mot Skills 215–220Google Scholar
  24. Henriques DY, Crawford JD (2002) Role of eye, head, and shoulder geometry in the planning of accurate arm movements. J Neurophysiol 87:1677–1685Google Scholar
  25. Henriques DY, Klier EM, Smith MA, Lowy D, Crawford JD (1998) Gaze-centered remapping of remembered visual space in an open-loop pointing task. J Neurosci 18:1583–1594Google Scholar
  26. Keele SW (1986) Motor control. In Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP (eds) Handbook of perception and performance. Wiley, New York, pp 1–60Google Scholar
  27. Kurata K (1993) Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movement-related activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. J Neurophysiol 69:187–200PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Laabs GJ (1973) Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short-term memory. J Exp Psychol 100:168–177PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Lemay M, Proteau L (2001) A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses. Exp Brain Res 140:357–368CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Lemay M, Proteau L (2002) The effects of target presentation time, recall delay and aging on the accuracy of manual pointing to remembered targets. J Mot Behav 34:11–23Google Scholar
  31. Logie RH (1995) Visuo-spatial working memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove, UKGoogle Scholar
  32. Logie RH, Marchetti C (1991) Visuo-spatial working memory: visual, spatial or central executive?. In: Logie RH, Denis M (eds) Mental images in human cognition. North Holland Press, Amsterdam, pp 105–115Google Scholar
  33. McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1997) Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space [published errata appear in J Neurophysiol 1998 Mar,79(3):preceding 1135 and 1998 Jun;79(6):3301]. J Neurophysiol 78:1601–1618PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1998) Short-term memory for reaching to visual targets: psychophysical evidence for body-centered reference frames. J Neurosci 18:8423–8435Google Scholar
  35. Messier J, Kalaska JF (1997) Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 115:469–478PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Neggers SF, Bekkering H (1999) Integration of visual and somatosensory target information in goal-directed eye and arm movements. Exp Brain Res 125:97–107CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Pepper RL, Herman LM (1970) Decay and interference effects in the short-term retention of a discrete motor act. J Exp Psychol 83:1–18Google Scholar
  39. Prablanc C, Echallier JF, Komilis E, Jeannerod M (1979) Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. I. Spatio-temporal characteristics of eye and hand movements and their relationship when varying the amount of visual information. Biol Cybern 35:113–124PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Quinn JG, Ralston GE (1986) Movement and attention in visual working memory. Q J Exp Psychol A 38:689–703Google Scholar
  41. Romero DH, Van Gemmert AWA, Adler CH, Bekkering H, Stelmach GE (2003) Altered aiming movements in Parkinson’s patients and elderly adults as a function of delays in movement onset. Exp Brain Res 151:249–261Google Scholar
  42. Rosenbaum DA (1980) Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:444–474CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Smyth MM, Pendleton LR (1989) Working memory for movements. Q J Exp Psychol A 41:235–250Google Scholar
  44. Soechting JF, Flanders M (1989a) Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 62:582–594Google Scholar
  45. Soechting JF, Flanders M (1989b) Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. J Neurophysiol 62:595–608PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Soechting JF, Helms-Tillery SI, Flanders M (1990) Transformation from head-centered to shoulder-centered representation of target direction in arm movements. J Cogn Neurosci 2:32–43Google Scholar
  47. Vindras P, Viviani P (1998) Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:569–591CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Vindras P, Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Viviani P (1998) Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. J Neurophysiol 79:3290–3294Google Scholar
  49. Wann JP, Ibrahim SF (1992) Does limb proprioception drift? Exp Brain Res 91:162–166CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Cognitive Neuroscience CenterUniversité du Québec à MontréalMontréalCanada
  2. 2.Motor Control LaboratoryArizona State UniversityTempeUSA

Personalised recommendations